Albert Bandura’s Social Cognitive Theory

Charlotte Nickerson

Research Assistant at Harvard University

Undergraduate at Harvard University

Charlotte Nickerson is a student at Harvard University obsessed with the intersection of mental health, productivity, and design.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Saul Mcleod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Key Takeaways

  • Social cognitive theory emphasizes the learning that occurs within a social context. In this view, people are active agents who can both influence and are influenced by their environment.
  • The theory was founded most prominently by Albert Bandura, who is also known for his work on observational learning, self-efficacy, and reciprocal determinism.
  • One assumption of social learning is that we learn new behaviors by observing the behavior of others and the consequences of their behavior.
  • If the behavior is rewarded (positive or negative reinforcement), we are likely to imitate it; however, if the behavior is punished, imitation is less likely. For example, in Bandura and Walters’ experiment,  the children imitated more the aggressive behavior of the model who was praised for being aggressive to the Bobo doll.
  • Social cognitive theory has been used to explain a wide range of human behavior, ranging from positive to negative social behaviors such as aggression, substance abuse, and mental health problems.

social cognitive theory 1

How We Learn From the Behavior of Others

Social cognitive theory views people as active agents who can both influence and are influenced by their environment.

The theory is an extension of social learning that includes the effects of cognitive processes — such as conceptions, judgment, and motivation — on an individual’s behavior and on the environment that influences them.

Rather than passively absorbing knowledge from environmental inputs, social cognitive theory argues that people actively influence their learning by interpreting the outcomes of their actions, which, in turn, affects their environments and personal factors, informing and altering subsequent behavior (Schunk, 2012).

By including thought processes in human psychology, social cognitive theory is able to avoid the assumption made by radical behaviorism that all human behavior is learned through trial and error. Instead, Bandura highlights the role of observational learning and imitation in human behavior.

Numerous psychologists, such as Julian Rotter and the American personality psychologist Walter Mischel, have proposed different social-cognitive perspectives.

Albert Bandura (1989) introduced the most prominent perspective on social cognitive theory.

Bandura’s perspective has been applied to a wide range of topics, such as personality development and functioning, the understanding and treatment of psychological disorders, organizational training programs, education, health promotion strategies, advertising and marketing, and more.

The central tenet of Bandura’s social-cognitive theory is that people seek to develop a sense of agency and exert control over the important events in their lives.

This sense of agency and control is affected by factors such as self-efficacy, outcome expectations, goals, and self-evaluation (Schunk, 2012).

Origins: The Bobo Doll Experiments

Social cognitive theory can trace its origins to Bandura and his colleagues, in particular, a series of well-known studies on observational learning known as the Bobo Doll experiments .

bobo doll experiment

In these experiments, researchers exposed young, preschool-aged children to videos of an adult acting violently toward a large, inflatable doll.

This aggressive behavior included verbal insults and physical violence, such as slapping and punching. At the end of the video, the children either witnessed the aggressor being rewarded, or punished or received no consequences for his behavior (Schunk, 2012).

After being exposed to this model, the children were placed in a room where they were given the same inflatable Bobo doll.

The researchers found that those who had watched the model either received positive reinforcement or no consequences for attacking the doll were more likely to show aggressive behavior toward the doll (Schunk, 2012).

This experiment was notable for being one that introduced the concept of observational learning to humans.

Bandura’s ideas about observational learning were in stark contrast to those of previous behaviorists, such as B.F. Skinner.

According to Skinner (1950), learning can only be achieved through individual action.

However, Bandura claimed that people and animals can also learn by watching and imitating the models they encounter in their environment, enabling them to acquire information more quickly.

Observational Learning

Bandura agreed with the behaviorists that behavior is learned through experience. However, he proposed a different mechanism than conditioning.

He argued that we learn through observation and imitation of others’ behavior.

This theory focuses not only on the behavior itself but also on the mental processes involved in learning, so it is not a pure behaviorist theory.

Social Learning Theory Bandura four stages mediation process in social learning theory attention retention motor reproduction motivation in diagram flat style.

Stages of the Social Learning Theory (SLT)

Not all observed behaviors are learned effectively. There are several factors involving both the model and the observer that determine whether or not a behavior is learned. These include attention, retention, motor reproduction, and motivation (Bandura & Walters, 1963).

The individual needs to pay attention to the behavior and its consequences and form a mental representation of the behavior. Some of the things that influence attention involve characteristics of the model.

This means that the model must be salient or noticeable. If the model is attractive, prestigious, or appears to be particularly competent, you will pay more attention. And if the model seems more like yourself, you pay more attention.

Storing the observed behavior in LTM where it can stay for a long period of time. Imitation is not always immediate. This process is often mediated by symbols. Symbols are “anything that stands for something else” (Bandura, 1998).

They can be words, pictures, or even gestures. For symbols to be effective, they must be related to the behavior being learned and must be understood by the observer.

Motor Reproduction

The individual must be able (have the ability and skills) to physically reproduce the observed behavior. This means that the behavior must be within their capability. If it is not, they will not be able to learn it (Bandura, 1998).

The observer must be motivated to perform the behavior. This motivation can come from a variety of sources, such as a desire to achieve a goal or avoid punishment.

Bandura (1977) proposed that motivation has three main components: expectancy, value, and affective reaction. Firstly, expectancy refers to the belief that one can successfully perform the behavior. Secondly, value refers to the importance of the goal that the behavior is meant to achieve.

The last of these, Affective reaction, refers to the emotions associated with the behavior.

If behavior is associated with positive emotions, it is more likely to be learned than a behavior associated with negative emotions. Reinforcement and punishment each play an important role in motivation.

Individuals must expect to receive the same positive reinforcement (vicarious reinforcement) for imitating the observed behavior that they have seen the model receiving.

Imitation is more likely to occur if the model (the person who performs the behavior) is positively reinforced. This is called vicarious reinforcement.

Imitation is also more likely if we identify with the model. We see them as sharing some characteristics with us, i.e., similar age, gender, and social status, as we identify with them.

Features of Social Cognitive Theory

The goal of social cognitive theory is to explain how people regulate their behavior through control and reinforcement in order to achieve goal-directed behavior that can be maintained over time.

Bandura, in his original formulation of the related social learning theory, included five constructs, adding self-efficacy to his final social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986).

Reciprocal Determinism

Reciprocal determinism is the central concept of social cognitive theory and refers to the dynamic and reciprocal interaction of people — individuals with a set of learned experiences — the environment, external social context, and behavior — the response to stimuli to achieve goals.

Its main tenet is that people seek to develop a sense of agency and exert control over the important events in their lives.

This sense of agency and control is affected by factors such as self-efficacy, outcome expectations, goals, and self-evaluation (Bandura, 1989).

To illustrate the concept of reciprocal determinism, Consider A student who believes they have the ability to succeed on an exam (self-efficacy) is more likely to put forth the necessary effort to study (behavior).

If they do not believe they can pass the exam, they are less likely to study. As a result, their beliefs about their abilities (self-efficacy) will be affirmed or disconfirmed by their actual performance on the exam (outcome).

This, in turn, will affect future beliefs and behavior. If the student passes the exam, they are likely to believe they can do well on future exams and put forth the effort to study.

If they fail, they may doubt their abilities (Bandura, 1989).

Behavioral Capability

Behavioral capability, meanwhile, refers to a person’s ability to perform a behavior by means of using their own knowledge and skills.

That is to say, in order to carry out any behavior, a person must know what to do and how to do it. People learn from the consequences of their behavior, further affecting the environment in which they live (Bandura, 1989).

Reinforcements

Reinforcements refer to the internal or external responses to a person’s behavior that affect the likelihood of continuing or discontinuing the behavior.

These reinforcements can be self-initiated or in one’s environment either positive or negative. Positive reinforcements increase the likelihood of a behavior being repeated, while negative reinforcers decrease the likelihood of a behavior being repeated.

Reinforcements can also be either direct or indirect. Direct reinforcements are an immediate consequence of a behavior that affects its likelihood, such as getting a paycheck for working (positive reinforcement).

Indirect reinforcements are not immediate consequences of behavior but may affect its likelihood in the future, such as studying hard in school to get into a good college (positive reinforcement) (Bandura, 1989).

Expectations

Expectations, meanwhile, refer to the anticipated consequences that a person has of their behavior.

Outcome expectations, for example, could relate to the consequences that someone foresees an action having on their health.

As people anticipate the consequences of their actions before engaging in a behavior, these expectations can influence whether or not someone completes the behavior successfully (Bandura, 1989).

Expectations largely come from someone’s previous experience. Nonetheless, expectancies also focus on the value that is placed on the outcome, something that is subjective from individual to individual.

For example, a student who may not be motivated to achieve high grades may place a lower value on taking the steps necessary to achieve them than someone who strives to be a high performer.

Self-Efficacy

Self-efficacy refers to the level of a person’s confidence in their ability to successfully perform a behavior.

Self-efficacy is influenced by a person’s own capabilities as well as other individual and environmental factors.

These factors are called barriers and facilitators (Bandura, 1989). Self-efficacy is often said to be task-specific, meaning that people can feel confident in their ability to perform one task but not another.

For example, a student may feel confident in their ability to do well on an exam but not feel as confident in their ability to make friends.

This is because self-efficacy is based on past experience and beliefs. If a student has never made friends before, they are less likely to believe that they will do so in the future.

Modeling Media and Social Cognitive Theory

Learning would be both laborious and hazardous in a world that relied exclusively on direct experience.

Social modeling provides a way for people to observe the successes and failures of others with little or no risk.

This modeling can take place on a massive scale. Modeling media is defined as “any type of mass communication—television, movies, magazines, music, etc.—that serves as a model for observing and imitating behavior” (Bandura, 1998).

In other words, it is a means by which people can learn new behaviors. Modeling media is often used in the fashion and taste industries to influence the behavior of consumers.

This is because modeling provides a reference point for observers to imitate. When done effectively, modeling can prompt individuals to adopt certain behaviors that they may not have otherwise engaged in.

Additionally, modeling media can provide reinforcement for desired behaviors.

For example, if someone sees a model wearing a certain type of clothing and receives compliments for doing so themselves, they may be more likely to purchase clothing like that of the model.

Observational Learning Examples

There are numerous examples of observational learning in everyday life for people of all ages.

Nonetheless, observational learning is especially prevalent in the socialization of children. For example:

  • A newer employee avoids being late to work after seeing a colleague be fired for being late.
  • A new store customer learns the process of lining up and checking out by watching other customers.
  • A traveler to a foreign country learning how to buy a ticket for a train and enter the gates by witnessing others do the same.
  • A customer in a clothing store learns the procedure for trying on clothes by watching others.
  • A person in a coffee shop learns where to find cream and sugar by watching other coffee drinkers locate the area.
  •  A new car salesperson learning how to approach potential customers by watching others.
  •  Someone moving to a new climate and learning how to properly remove snow from his car and driveway by seeing his neighbors do the same.
  •  A tenant learning to pay rent on time as a result of seeing a neighbor evicted for late payment.
  •  An inexperienced salesperson becomes successful at a sales meeting or in giving a presentation after observing the behaviors and statements of other salespeople.
  •  A viewer watches an online video to learn how to contour and shape their eyebrows and then goes to the store to do so themselves.
  •  Drivers slow down after seeing that another driver has been pulled over by a police officer.
  •  A bank teller watches their more efficient colleague in order to learn a more efficient way of counting money.
  •  A shy party guest watching someone more popular talk to different people in the crowd, later allowing them to do the same thing.
  • Adult children behave in the same way that their parents did when they were young.
  • A lost student navigating a school campus after seeing others do it on their own.

Social Learning vs. Social Cognitive Theory

Social learning theory and Social Cognitive Theory are both theories of learning that place an emphasis on the role of observational learning.

However, there are several key differences between the two theories. Social learning theory focuses on the idea of reinforcement, while Social Cognitive Theory emphasizes the role of cognitive processes.

Additionally, social learning theory posits that all behavior is learned through observation, while Social Cognitive Theory allows for the possibility of learning through other means, such as direct experience.

Finally, social learning theory focuses on individualistic learning, while Social Cognitive Theory takes a more holistic view, acknowledging the importance of environmental factors.

Though they are similar in many ways, the differences between social learning theory and Social Cognitive Theory are important to understand. These theories provide different frameworks for understanding how learning takes place.

As such, they have different implications in all facets of their applications (Reed et al., 2010).

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory . Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84 (2), 191.

 Bandura, A. (1986). Fearful expectations and avoidant actions as coeffects of perceived self-inefficacy.

Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American psychologist, 44 (9), 1175.

Bandura, A. (1998). Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychology and health, 13 (4), 623-649.

Bandura, A. (2003). Social cognitive theory for personal and social change by enabling media. In Entertainment-education and social change (pp. 97-118). Routledge.

Bandura, A. Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through the imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology , 63, 575-582.

LaMort, W. (2019). The Social Cognitive Theory. Boston University.

Reed, M. S., Evely, A. C., Cundill, G., Fazey, I., Glass, J., Laing, A., … & Stringer, L. C. (2010). What is social learning?. Ecology and society, 15 (4).

Schunk, D. H. (2012). Social cognitive theory .

Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary?. Psychological Review, 57 (4), 193.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Related Articles

Aversion Therapy & Examples of Aversive Conditioning

Learning Theories

Aversion Therapy & Examples of Aversive Conditioning

Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory

Learning Theories , Psychology , Social Science

Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory

Behaviorism In Psychology

Learning Theories , Psychology

Behaviorism In Psychology

Bandura’s Bobo Doll Experiment on Social Learning

Famous Experiments , Learning Theories

Bandura’s Bobo Doll Experiment on Social Learning

Bloom’s Taxonomy of Learning

Bloom’s Taxonomy of Learning

Jerome Bruner’s Theory Of Learning And Cognitive Development

Child Psychology , Learning Theories

Jerome Bruner’s Theory Of Learning And Cognitive Development

Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane Australia

Case study: using social cognitive theory and social support coping theory to improve breastfeeding duration rates: MumBubConnect

Russell-Bennett, Rebekah , Gallegos, Danielle , Previte, Josephine , & Hamilton, Robyn (2014) Case study: using social cognitive theory and social support coping theory to improve breastfeeding duration rates: MumBubConnect. In Aleti, T , Binney, W , Parker, L , Nguyen, D , & Brennan, L (Eds.) Social marketing and behaviour change: Models, theory and applications. Edward Elgar Publishing, United Kingdom, pp. 66-74.

View at publisher

Description

In this case study, social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986, 2004) and social support as a way of coping (Vitaliano et al., 1985) have been selected to overcome barriers created by low levels of self-confidence and perceived lack of support in the context of breastfeeding. Thus, the application of the two theories in this case study is designed to improve the mothers’ self-efficacy and reinforce the behaviour of breastfeeding. The World Health Organization (WHO) recommends exclusive breastfeeding for the first six months of life (WHO, 2001); however, in Australia (as with many developed countries), breastfeeding duration declines rapidly after three months. A total of 47 per cent of infants are fully breastfed to three months, reducing to 21 per cent being predominantly breastfed to five months (Australian Institute of Health and Welfare [AIHW], 2011a). The national significance of breastfeeding is noted with the release of the National Breastfeeding Strategy in late 2009, which aimed to improve the health of infants, young children and mothers by protecting, promoting, supporting and monitoring breastfeeding (National Health and Medical Research Council [NHMRC], 2013). It is critical that Australia addresses the poor continuation of breastfeeding to protect the next generation of Australians against acute and chronic diseases. A social marketing programme was therefore developed that aimed to test the effect of a technology-based intervention on breastfeeding duration. The intervention was conducted in Australia with participants from every state.

Impact and interest:

Citation counts are sourced monthly from Scopus and Web of Science® citation databases.

These databases contain citations from different subsets of available publications and different time periods and thus the citation count from each is usually different. Some works are not in either database and no count is displayed. Scopus includes citations from articles published in 1996 onwards, and Web of Science® generally from 1980 onwards.

Citations counts from the Google Scholar™ indexing service can be viewed at the linked Google Scholar™ search.

  • Notify us of incorrect data
  • How to use citation counts
  • More information

Export: EndNote | Dublin Core | BibTeX

Repository Staff Only: item control page

-

  • Browse research
  • TEQSA Provider ID: PRV12079 (Australian University)
  • CRICOS No. 00213J
  • ABN 83 791 724 622
  • Accessibility
  • Right to Information

Social Cognitive Theory

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online: 22 April 2024
  • Cite this living reference work entry

case study social cognitive theory

  • Shen Decan 2 &
  • Zhang Kan 3  

Social Cognitive Theory is a social psychological theory that aims to reveal how individuals’ internal knowledge structure and belief system explain and give meaning to social objects and their interrelationships. According to Gestalt psychology, the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. Therefore, an understanding of the whole requires a top-down analysis from the overall structure to the characteristics of every part. In the 1930s and 1940s, Kurt Lewin broke new ground in the study of Gestalt psychology by founding topological psychology that focuses on the study of will and need. He proposed an equation for behavior, B = f ( P, E ), emphasizing that behavior ( B ) is a function of two factors: the person ( P ) and their environment ( E ). That is to say, behavior changes with the change of person and social environment. Lewin’s contemporaries, such as Fritz Heider, Muzafer Sherif, Solomon Asch, and Theodore Newcomb, also made great progress in studying cognitive balance, formation of...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Further Reading

Aronson E, Wilson TD, Akert RM (2014) Social psychology, 8th edn. Pearson India Education Services Pvt. Ltd, Chennai

Google Scholar  

Yue G-A (2013) Social psychology, 2nd edn. China Renmin University Press, Beijing

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, Beijing, China

Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), Beijing, China

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zhang Kan .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2024 The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this entry

Cite this entry.

Decan, S., Kan, Z. (2024). Social Cognitive Theory. In: The ECPH Encyclopedia of Psychology. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-6000-2_826-1

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-6000-2_826-1

Received : 23 March 2024

Accepted : 25 March 2024

Published : 22 April 2024

Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore

Print ISBN : 978-981-99-6000-2

Online ISBN : 978-981-99-6000-2

eBook Packages : Springer Reference Behavioral Science and Psychology Reference Module Humanities and Social Sciences Reference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Social Cognitive Theory: How We Learn From the Behavior of Others

Thomas Barwick/Getty Images 

  • Archaeology

case study social cognitive theory

  • Ph.D., Psychology, Fielding Graduate University
  • M.A., Psychology, Fielding Graduate University
  • B.A., Film Studies, Cornell University

Social cognitive theory is a learning theory developed by the renowned Stanford psychology professor Albert Bandura. The theory provides a framework for understanding how people actively shape and are shaped by their environment. In particular, the theory details the processes of observational learning and modeling, and the influence of self-efficacy on the production of behavior.

Key Takeaways: Social Cognitive Theory

  • Social cognitive theory was developed by Stanford psychologist Albert Bandura.
  • The theory views people as active agents who both influence and are influenced by their environment.
  • A major component of the theory is observational learning: the process of learning desirable and undesirable behaviors by observing others, then reproducing learned behaviors in order to maximize rewards.
  • Individuals' beliefs in their own self-efficacy influences whether or not they will reproduce an observed behavior.

Origins: The Bobo Doll Experiments

In the 1960s, Bandura, along with his colleagues, initiated a series of well-known studies on observational learning called the Bobo Doll experiments. In the first of these experiments , pre-school children were exposed to an aggressive or nonaggressive adult model to see if they would imitate the model’s behavior. The gender of the model was also varied, with some children observing same-sex models and some observing opposite-sex models.

In the aggressive condition, the model was verbally and physically aggressive towards an inflated Bobo doll in the presence of the child. After exposure to the model, the child was taken to another room to play with a selection of highly attractive toys. To frustrate participants, the child’s play was stopped after about two minutes. At that point, the child was taken to a third room filled with different toys, including a Bobo doll, where they were allowed to play for the next 20 minutes.

The researchers found that the children in the aggressive condition were much more likely to display verbal and physical aggression, including aggression towards the Bobo doll and other forms of aggression. In addition, boys were more likely to be aggressive than girls, especially if they had been exposed to an aggressive male model.

A subsequent experiment utilized a similar protocol, but in this case, the aggressive models weren’t just seen in real-life. There was also a second group that observed a film of the aggressive model as well as a third group that observed a film of an aggressive cartoon character. Again, the gender of the model was varied, and the children were subjected to mild frustration before they were brought to the experimental room to play. As in the previous experiment, the children in the three aggressive conditions exhibited more aggressive behavior than those in the control group and boys in the aggressive condition exhibiting more aggression than girls.

These studies served as the basis for ideas about observational learning and modeling both in real-life and through the media. In particular, it spurred a debate over the ways media models can negatively influence children that continues today. 

In 1977, Bandura introduced Social Learning Theory, which further refined his ideas on observational learning and modeling. Then in 1986, Bandura renamed his theory Social Cognitive Theory in order to put greater emphasis on the cognitive components of observational learning and the way behavior, cognition, and the environment interact to shape people.

Observational Learning

A major component of social cognitive theory is observational learning. Bandura’s ideas about learning stood in contrast to those of behaviorists like B.F. Skinner . According to Skinner, learning could only be achieved by taking individual action. However, Bandura claimed that observational learning, through which people observe and imitate models they encounter in their environment, enables people to acquire information much more quickly.

Observational learning occurs through a sequence of four processes :

  • Attentional processes account for the information that is selected for observation in the environment. People might select to observe real-life models or models they encounter via media.
  • Retention processes involve remembering the observed information so it can be successfully recalled and reconstructed later.
  • Production processes reconstruct the memories of the observations so what was learned can be applied in appropriate situations. In many cases, this doesn’t mean the observer will replicate the observed action exactly, but that they will modify the behavior to produce a variation that fits the context.
  • Motivational processes determine whether or not an observed behavior is performed based on whether that behavior was observed to result in desired or adverse outcomes for the model. If an observed behavior was rewarded, the observer will be more motivated to reproduce it later. However, if a behavior was punished in some way, the observer would be less motivated to reproduce it. Thus, social cognitive theory cautions that people don’t perform every behavior they learn through modeling.

Self-Efficacy

In addition to the information models can convey during observational learning, models can also increase or decrease the observer’s belief in their self-efficacy to enact observed behaviors and bring about desired outcomes from those behaviors. When people see others like them succeed, they also believe they can be capable of succeeding. Thus, models are a source of motivation and inspiration.

Perceptions of self-efficacy influence people’s choices and beliefs in themselves, including the goals they choose to pursue and the effort they put into them, how long they’re willing to persevere in the face of obstacles and setbacks, and the outcomes they expect. Thus, self-efficacy influences one’s motivations to perform various actions and one's belief in their ability to do so.

Such beliefs can impact personal growth and change. For example, research has shown that enhancing self-efficacy beliefs is more likely to result in the improvement of health habits than the use of fear-based communication. Belief in one’s self-efficacy can be the difference between whether or not an individual even considers making positive changes in their life.

Modeling Media

The prosocial potential of media models has been demonstrated through serial dramas that were produced for developing communities on issues such as literacy, family planning, and the status of women. These dramas have been successful in bringing about positive social change, while demonstrating the relevance and applicability of social cognitive theory to media.

For example, a television show in India was produced to raise women’s status and promote smaller families by embedding these ideas in the show. The show championed gender equality by including characters that positively modeled women’s equality. In addition, there were other characters that modeled subservient women’s roles and some that transitioned between subservience and equality. The show was popular, and despite its melodramatic narrative, viewers understood the messages it modeled. These viewers learned that women should have equal rights, should have the freedom to choose how they live their lives, and be able to limit the size of their families. In this example and others, the tenets of social cognitive theory have been utilized to make a positive impact through fictional media models.

  • Bandura, Albert. “Social cognitive theory for personal and social change by enabling media.” Entertainment-education and social change: History, research, and practice , edited by Arvind Singhal, Michael J. Cody, Everett M. Rogers, and Miguel Sabido, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2004, pp. 75-96.
  • Bandura, Albert. “Social Cognitive Theory of Mass Communication. Media Psychology , vol. 3, no. 3, 2001, pp. 265-299, https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0303_03
  • Bandura, Albert. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory . Prentice Hall, 1986.
  • Bandura, Albert, Dorothea Ross, and Sheila A. Ross. “Transmission of Aggression Through Imitation of Aggressive Models.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 63, no. 3, 1961, pp. 575-582, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0045925
  • Bandura, Albert, Dorothea Ross, and Sheila A. Ross. “Imitation of Film-Mediated Aggressive Models.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 66, no. 1, 1961, pp. 3-11, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0048687
  • Crain, William. Theories of Development: Concepts and Applications . 5th ed., Pearson Prentice Hall, 2005.
  • Understanding Self-Efficacy
  • What Is Gender Socialization? Definition and Examples
  • What Is Social Learning Theory?
  • Information Processing Theory: Definition and Examples
  • Sutherland's Differential Association Theory Explained
  • Cognitive Dissonance Theory: Definition and Examples
  • Gender Schema Theory Explained
  • How Psychology Defines and Explains Deviant Behavior
  • What Is Role Strain? Definition and Examples
  • What Is Behaviorism in Psychology?
  • What Is Belief Perseverance? Definition and Examples
  • Major Sociological Theories
  • Cultivation Theory
  • What Is Uses and Gratifications Theory? Definition and Examples
  • Attribution Theory: The Psychology of Interpreting Behavior
  • Rational Choice Theory

Logo for Open Oregon Educational Resources

3 Social Cognitive Theory

At the end of this chapter, you will be able to:

  • Identify key elements of social cognitive theory
  • Explain strategies utilized to implement social cognitive theory
  • Summarize the criticisms of social cognitive theory and educational implications
  • Explain how equity is impacted by social cognitive theory
  • Identify classroom strategies to support the use of social cognitive theory
  • Select strategies to support student success utilizing social cognitive theory
  • Develop a plan to implement the use of social cognitive theory ​​

SCENARIO: Yesterday, Ms Mitchell felt exhausted at the end of the school day so today she was going to try something new. In her science class, the students could not seem to follow the neatly printed directions on the white board, nor did the color-coordinated handouts seem to make any difference. She had run around the room trying to respond to the different groups attempting the science project but there was a great deal of confusion. It was one of those days where she questioned her career choice- was she really cut out to be a teacher? After a good night’s sleep and coaching from a colleague, Ms. Mitchell was determined to try a different approach and model every step of the process. After she modeled each section, students seemed to get it quickly.  Following some verbal encouragement from Ms. Mitchell, there was soon a happy buzz in the classroom as students engaged with each other in the steps of the science project. Ms. Mitchell was even able to rest her feet, drink her herbal tea and consider what a difference these simple strategies made.

What changes did Ms. Mitchell make? How did modeling the activity change the end result and facilitate their learning? How did her positive remarks reinforce their confidence with the tasks? How did group work also build engagement? As you read through this chapter, consider the power of observation and how learning occurs in a social context with a dynamic and reciprocal interaction of the person, environment, and behavior.

Video 3.1 – Social Cognitive Theory

Introduction.

Albert Bandura (1925-2021) was born in Mundare, Alberta, Canada, the youngest of six children. Both of his parents were immigrants from Eastern Europe. Bandura’s father worked as a track layer for the Trans-Canada railroad while his mother worked in a general store before they were able to buy some land and become farmers. Though times were often hard growing up, Bandura’s parents placed great emphasis on celebrating life and more importantly family. They were also very keen on their children doing well in school. Mundare had only one school at the time so Bandura did all of his schooling in one place.

Bandura attended the University of British Columbia and graduated three years later in 1949 with the Bolocan Award in psychology. Bandura then went to the University of Iowa to complete his graduate work. At the time, the University of Iowa was central to psychological study, especially in the area of social learning theory. By 1952, Bandura completed his Master’s and Ph.D. in clinical psychology. Bandura worked at the Wichita Guidance Center before accepting a position as a faculty member at Stanford University in 1953. Bandura has studied many different topics over the years, including aggression in adolescents (more specifically he was interested in aggression in boys who came from intact middle-class families), children’s abilities to self-regulate and self-reflect, and of course self-efficacy (a person’s perception and beliefs about their ability to produce effects, or influence events that concern their lives).

Bandura is perhaps most famous for his Bobo Doll experiments  in the 1960s. At the time there was a popular belief that learning was a result of reinforcement. In the Bobo Doll experiments, Bandura presented children with social models of novel (new) violent behavior or non-violent behavior towards the inflatable rebounding Bobo Doll.

case study social cognitive theory

  As children continue through adolescence toward adulthood, they need to assume responsibility for themselves in all aspects of life. They must master many new skills, and a sense of confidence in working toward the future is dependent on a developing sense of self-efficacy supported by past experiences of mastery. In adulthood, a healthy and realistic sense of self-efficacy provides the motivation necessary to pursue success in one’s life.

  In summary, as we learn more about our world and how it works, we also learn that we can have a significant impact on it. Most importantly, we can have a direct effect on our immediate personal environment, especially with regard to personal relationships, behaviors, and goals. What motivates us to try influencing our environment is specific ways in which we believe, indeed, we can make a difference in a direction we want in life. Thus, research has focused largely on what people think about their efficacy, rather than on their actual ability to achieve their goals (Bandura, 1997).

Impact of Social Cognitive Theory

Bandura is still influencing the world with expansions of Social Cognitive Theory (SCT). SCT has been applied to many areas of human functioning such as career choice and organizational behavior as well as in understanding classroom motivation, learning, and achievement (Lent, Brown, & Hackett, 1994). Bandura (2001) brought SCT to mass communication in his journal article that stated the theory could be used to analyze how “symbolic communication influences human thought, affect and action” (p. 3). The theory shows how new behavior diffuses through society by psychosocial factors governing acquisition and adoption of the behavior. Bandura’s (2011) book chapter “The Social and Policy Impact of Social Cognitive Theory” to extend SCT’s application in health promotion and urgent global issues, which provides insight into addressing global problems through a macro social lens, aiming at improving equality of individuals’ lives under the umbrellas of SCT. This work focuses on how SCT impacts areas of both health and population effects in relation to climate change. He proposes that these problems could be solved through television serial dramas that show models similar to viewers performing the desired behavior.

Bandura (2011) states population growth is a global crisis because of its correlation with depletion and degradation of our planet’s resources. Bandura argues that SCT should be used to get people to use birth control, reduce gender inequality through education, and to model environmental conservation to improve the state of the planet. Green and Peil (2009) reported he has tried to use cognitive theory to solve a number of global problems such as environmental conservation, poverty, soaring population growth, etc.

Criticism of Social Cognitive Theory

  • The social cognitive theory is that it is not a unified theory. This means that the different aspects of the theory may not be connected. For example, researchers currently cannot find a connection between observational learning and self-efficacy within the social-cognitive perspective.
  • The theory is so broad that not all of its component parts are fully understood and integrated into a single explanation of learning.  The findings associated with this theory are still, for the most part, preliminary.
  • The theory is limited in that not all social learning can be directly observed. Because of this, it can be difficult to quantify the effect that social cognition has on development.
  • Finally, this theory tends to ignore maturation throughout the lifespan. Because of this, the understanding of how a child learns through observation and how an adult learns through observation are not differentiated, and factors of development are not included.

  Image 3.7

Educational implications of social cognitive theory.

An important assumption of Social Cognitive Theory is that personal determinants, such as self-reflection and self-regulation, do not have to reside unconsciously within individuals . People can consciously change and develop their cognitive functioning. This is important to the proposition that self-efficacy too can be changed, or enhanced. From this perspective, people are capable of influencing their own motivation and performance according to the model of triadic reciprocality in which personal determinants (such as self-efficacy), environmental conditions (such as treatment conditions), and action (such as practice) are mutually interactive influences. Improving performance, therefore, depends on changing some of these influences.

Relevancy to the classroom:

  In teaching and learning, the challenge upfront is to:

  • Get the learner to believe in his or her personal capabilities to successfully perform a designated task.
  • Provide environmental conditions, such as instructional strategies and appropriate technology, that improve the strategies and self-efficacy of the learner.
  • Provide opportunities for the learner to experience successful learning as a result of appropriate action (Self-efficacy Theory, n.d.).

case study social cognitive theory

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Digit Health
  • v.4; Jan-Dec 2018

Social cognitive determinants of exercise behavior in the context of behavior modeling: a mixed method approach

Short abstract.

Research has shown that persuasive technologies aimed at behavior change will be more effective if behavioral determinants are targeted. However, research on the determinants of bodyweight exercise performance in the context of behavior modeling in fitness apps is scarce. To bridge this gap, we conducted an empirical study among 659 participants resident in North America using social cognitive theory as a framework to uncover the determinants of the performance of bodyweight exercise behavior. To contextualize our study, we modeled, in a hypothetical context, two popular bodyweight exercise behaviors – push ups and squats – featured in most fitness apps on the market using a virtual coach (aka behavior model). Our social cognitive model shows that users’ perceived self-efficacy (β T  = 0.23, p  < 0.001) and perceived social support (β T  = 0.23, p  < 0.001) are the strongest determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior, followed by outcome expectation (β T  = 0.11, p  < 0.05). However, users’ perceived self-regulation (β T  = –0.07, p  = n.s.) turns out to be a non-determinant of bodyweight exercise behavior. Comparatively, our model shows that perceived self-efficacy has a stronger direct effect on exercise behavior for men (β = 0.31, p  < 0.001) than for women (β = 0.10, p  = n.s.). In contrast, perceived social support has a stronger direct effect on exercise behavior for women (β = 0.15, p  < 0.05) than for men (β = −0.01, p  = n.s.). Based on these findings and qualitative analysis of participants’ comments, we provide a set of guidelines for the design of persuasive technologies for promoting regular exercise behavior.

Introduction

Recent studies of the most popular topics in the health and fitness domain show that bodyweight exercises have gained traction and attention among health and fitness enthusiasts worldwide. For example, in a global survey of the trending health and fitness topics, bodyweight exercise has consistently featured in the top four positions of the fitness trend chart in the last 3 years. 1 – 3 Bodyweight exercise is the use of one’s body weight as resistance as opposed to free weights or exercise equipment during a workout. There are a number of reasons for its popularity worldwide. First, it is inexpensive in the sense that it does not require the owning of equipment or signing up to become a member of a gym. Second, it can be performed in the comfort of one’s home (e.g. bedroom, sitting room, etc.). Third, it can be done when one is away from home (e.g. in a hotel room). Finally, bodyweight exercise offers a number of health benefits, which include gaining of strength, building of muscles, improvement of cardiovascular fitness, and burning of fat. 4 , 5 Overall, bodyweight exercise is an effective way to improve balance, stability and flexibility. 6 Hence, it has become important for researchers to investigate its determinants with the specific aim of designing persuasive applications to support its performance anywhere and anytime.

In general, physical inactivity has been pin-pointed as one of the main causes of non-communicable diseases, such as strokes, hypertension, type 2 diabetes, etc., which account for 6% of global mortality annually. 7 Research has shown that most adults (within the age group of 18–64 years) worldwide do not meet the minimum recommendation of at least 150 minutes of moderate-intensity aerobic physical activity per week, or its equivalent of at least 75 minutes of vigorous-intensity aerobic physical activity per week. 7 , 8 Moreover, research has shown that, although most people are aware of the importance and benefits of physical activity, they lack the willpower or motivation to exercise regularly. 9 Apart from the lack of motivation, lack of time due to other priorities 10 and lack of access to recreational facilities, e.g. gym, 11 are among the main reasons why people do not exercise regularly. Thus, there is a need for health practitioners, researchers and persuasive technology (PT) designers to promote physical activity in the context of an individual’s circumstances and environment. 11 We argue that encouraging home-based bodyweight exercise might be one way to tackle the challenges of lack of time and access to physical activity facilities. The reason for this is that home-based bodyweight exercise does not require people to leave their home; neither does it require them to possess exercise equipment, which may be unaffordable to some people and could be a barrier to physical activity. 11 Research has shown that PTs can be used as an effective support system to motivate and facilitate positive behavior change in humans, 12 especially those who are willing and open to change. 13

Thus, to assist PT researchers and designers in developing well-informed behavior change support systems in this area, we conducted an empirical study of the social cognitive theory (SCT) determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior in the context of behavior modeling in a fitness app. In recent years, behavior modeling has been found to be one of the most commonly used behavior change techniques in most fitness apps in the marketplace. 14 By definition, behavior modeling entails the demonstration of the correct performance of a given behavior by an expert to an observer. 15 – 17 It is a form of vicarious modeling, which could be carried out in a real-life environment, such as a classroom, by a real person, or in a simulated (virtual) environment, such as a video, by a virtual coach or role model. On the other hand, SCT is one of the most widely applied behavioral theories for promoting health interventions. 18 The link between behavior modeling and SCT is ‘observational learning’. In particular, observational learning is at the core of the social learning theory (SLT), which later developed into the SCT. It posits that through behavior modeling, people are able to observe the performance of a given behavior and reproduce it subsequently. More specifically, it holds that ‘if individuals see successful demonstration of a behavior, they can also complete the behavior successfully’. 18 This is made possible through cognitive processes which motivate and/or mediate human behaviors. 19 However, in the context of behavior modeling in the fitness domain, there is limited research on how the core SCT factors, which are impacted by the perceived persuasiveness of behavior models, 20 in turn, influence exercise behavior performance.

To bridge this gap and advance the current research in this area of PT, we modeled the SCT determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior, using videos of behavior models performing push-ups and squats as a case study. Our study was based on a sample of 659 participants resident in North America. The results of our structural equation modeling (SEM) show that the observers’ perceived self-efficacy (β T  = 0.23, p  < 0.001) and perceived social support (β T  = 0.23, p  < 0.001) are the strongest determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior performance, followed by outcome expectation (β T  = 0.11, p  < 0.05). Perceived self-regulation (β T  =−0.07, p  = n.s.) turns out not to be a determinant of bodyweight exercise behavior performance. Comparatively, our SCT model shows that, for men, perceived self-efficacy is a stronger determinant (motivator) than perceived social support, while, for women, perceived social support is a stronger determinant than perceived self-efficacy. Finally, based on these findings and qualitative analysis of participants’ comments, we provide a set of design guidelines to help fitness app designers to develop more effective PT interventions in the fitness domain.

In this section, we provide an overview of SCT and observation learning. For brevity, in the rest of the paper, for the most part, we will omit the qualifier ‘perceived’ from the names of the SCT factors.

Social cognitive theory

SCT is a behavior theory of human motivation and action. It is an offshoot of the SLT 21 proposed by Bandura 22 to explain the various internal and external processes (cognitive, vicarious, self-reflective and self-regulatory) that come into play in human psychosocial functioning. It is organized within a causation framework known as the triadic reciprocal determinism, which states that cognitive, behavioral and environmental factors dynamically interact with one another in a reciprocal fashion to shape human behavior. 23 For example, with respect to exercise behavior, self-efficacy, self-regulation and outcome expectation are typical examples of cognitive factors which shape behavior, while social support is an example of environmental factors. Self-efficacy refers to the belief in one’s ability to perform a given behavior. It is regarded as the strongest (proximal) determinant of behavior change. 24 Self-regulation is the control and management of one’s behavior through planning, setting goals and self-monitoring of one’s performance. Outcome expectation is the belief one holds about the consequences of a behavior, which could be positive or negative. Finally, social support refers to the assistance people get from others towards the performance of a behavior. For the purpose of our paper, these theoretical determinants of behavior are examined at the level of perception in the context of behavior modeling aimed at motivating exercise behavior change.

Observational learning

Observational learning refers to the acquisition of knowledge through observation. According to Bandura, 22 ‘observational learning enables humans to develop their knowledge and skills through information conveyed by modeling influences’ (p. 25). SCT holds that much of human knowledge is acquired through observational learning. In particular, it states that people intentionally or unintentionally learn by observing the behaviors of others (models) and their consequences. Moreover, it holds that people may choose to replicate a behavior depending on whether they are rewarded or punished for it. Electronic technologies (e.g. television, radio, etc.) are examples of mass media through which behavior models can transmit new ways of thinking and behaving to a critical mass of people in the society at large with the aim of changing attitudes and behaviors. 22 In more recent times, social media and gamified PTs have become popular media, also known as socially influencing systems, 25 aimed at motivating behavior change, including engagement in targeted behaviors such as cycling, 26 healthy eating, 27 , 28 physical activity, 20 etc. In the context of our study, our simulated behavior models (in a prototyped fitness application), shown in Figure 1 , represent virtual social agents of change, 29 with the observers of the modeled exercise behavior being the targeted audience.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 10.1177_2055207618811555-fig1.jpg

Videos of behavior models demonstrating push-up and squat exercises. 20

Related work

A number of studies have been carried out with respect to the social cognitive model of behavior, using SEM analysis. We provide a cross-section of these studies in the domain of physical activity. Rovniak et al. 24 presented the social cognitive model of the physical activity of college students from Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University in the United States (USA). In their study, they measured self-efficacy, self-regulation, social support and outcome expectation at baseline and used them to predict physical activity 8 weeks later. They found that self-efficacy was the strongest determinants of physical activity, followed by self-regulation and social support. Similarly, Oyibo and colleagues 30 , 31 modeled the physical activity of two different college student populations in Canada and Nigeria using the SCT as a theoretical framework. The authors measured all of the four main determinants of physical activity and used it to predict participants’ reported level of physical activity in the past 7 days. They found that self-efficacy and self-regulation had the strongest total effect on physical activity among the Canadian group, while social support and body image had the strongest total effect on physical activity among the Nigerian group.

Resnick 32 presented a social cognitive model of the current exercise of older adults, living in a continuing care retirement community in the USA. The author found that self-efficacy, outcome expectation and prior exercise were among the strongest determinants of current exercise. Similarly, Anderson et al. 33 modeled the physical activity of adults from 14 southwestern Virginia churches in the USA using the SCT as a theoretical framework. They found that self-regulation, self-efficacy and social support are the strongest determinants of the adults’ physical activity. Moreover, Anderson-Bill et al. 34 investigated the determinants of physical activity among web-health users resident in the USA and Canada. Their model was based on the SCT and focused on walking as the target behavior. Specifically, they used pedometers to track participants’ daily steps and minutes walked over a 7-day period. They found that, overall, self-efficacy, self-regulation and social support were the determinants of participants’ physical activity, with self-efficacy being the strongest. Moreover, in the context of behavior modeling in a fitness app, Oyibo et al. 20 investigated the perceived effect of behavior modeling on the SCT factors. They found that the perceived persuasiveness of the exercise behavior model design has a significant direct effect on self-regulation, outcome expectation and self-efficacy. More specifically, the effect was stronger on the first two SCT factors than on the third.

The major limitation of the above studies, apart from the last one reviewed, is that most of them used convenience samples, especially the student population. This may affect generalizing to a more diverse population sample. 24 Moreover, none of the previous studies has investigated the SCT determinants of exercise behavior in the context of behavior modeling (in a fitness app), which is one of the main sources of self-efficacy 35 including the other core social cognitive factors. 20 Second, most previous studies did not use a mixed-method approach, comprising quantitative and qualitative analyses. Our study aims to fill this gap by using the mixed-method approach and providing evidence-based design guidelines for developing more effective fitness apps in the future.

This section covers our research question/design, measurement instruments and the demographics of study participants.

Research objective and design

The aim of our study is to investigate the determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior performance in the context of behavior modeling in fitness apps using the SCT as a theoretical framework. In particular, we aim to understand which of the SCT factors are the strongest drivers of exercise behavior performance and how the gender of the observers of the behavior moderates the various interrelationships among the SCT factors and the target behavior. More formally, our research questions can be stated as follows:

  • How are the SCT constructs interrelated in the context of behavior modeling in a fitness app?
  • Which of the SCT constructs are the strongest determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior performance?
  • Does gender moderate the interrelationships among the SCT constructs and exercise behavior performance?

To address the above research questions, we designed a hypothetical fitness app for encouraging exercise behavior on the home front. The app modeled two types of bodyweight exercise behaviors – push-ups and squats – that are commonly used in current fitness apps on the market. Apart from exercise type, the behavior models were designed taking race (black and white) and gender (male and female) into consideration. Figure 1 shows two of the eight versions of the behavior models, one of which was randomly administered to each participant who took part in the study. However, in this paper, we do not investigate the moderating effect of the design characteristics of the behavior models (i.e. race, gender and exercise type). In our survey, we requested participants to answer a number of SCT-related questions (see subsection Measurement instruments). Before answering the questions, the app was described to participants as follows:

Imagine you want to improve your personal health and fitness level. Given the challenges (e.g. time, cost, weather, etc.) associated with going to the gym regularly, the ‘Homex App’ has been created, say by health promoters in your neighborhood, to support your physical activity.

In particular, we intend to use the feedback from participants and the quantitative findings to inform our future PT intervention aimed at motivating people to exercise more (especially at home). Thus, in our survey, a snapshot of the mock-up of the proposed application (behavior models performing push-up and squat exercises shown in Figure 1 ) was presented to participants to elicit their feedback and investigate the interrelationships among the SCT factors and exercise behavior performance. Moreover, in this study, we assume that most of the respondents, with respect to susceptibility to persuasive technologies, are likely to be ‘January 1st’ people, who are open to change. Stibe and Larson 13 described this group of people as ‘the most welcoming towards technology supported behavioral interventions’ designed to facilitate the achievement of the target behavior.

Measurement instruments

We adapted our measurement instruments (see Table 1 ) from existing SCT scales in the literature. Exercise behavior was measured using the projected number of repetitions of bodyweight exercise (push-ups or squats) a participant could perform per week. Self-efficacy was measured using the scale proposed by Schwarzer and Renner. 36 Social support, outcome expectation and self-regulation were adapted from Sallis, 37 Wójcicki et al. 38 and Rovniak et al., 24 respectively. Self-efficacy and social support used a Likert scale ranging from ‘not confident (0)’ to ‘confident (100)’, while outcome expectation and self-regulation ranged from ‘strongly disagree (1)’ to ‘strongly agree (5)’. In the SEM and descriptive statistics analysis, the 1–5 scale was rescaled to the 0–100% scale to ensure uniformity. 39

Measurement instruments.

Outcome expectation comprises two lower-order constructs.Items 1 to 5 measure the physical outcome expectations, while items 6 to 8 measure the social outcome expectations.

Participants

We submitted our research questionnaire to the authors’ university’s behavioral research ethics board. After its approval, we recruited participants from Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT – a crowdsourcing platform based in the USA). We chose AMT because it is a platform through which researchers can gather data from diverse users with different demographic variables. Second, we chose AMT because it provides a mechanism that will allow for gathering research data that can be relied upon to a certain degree. For example, the platform allows researchers to reject responses they consider ‘poor’. This tends to reduce the chances of receiving invalid responses from questionnaire takers given the negative effect it has on their ‘overall reputation’ on the platform, which may prevent them from having the opportunity to participate in certain surveys in the future. This ‘quality assurance’ mechanism tends to increase the reliability of the gathered data on the platform compared to otherwise. In appreciation of participants’ time in taking the survey, which lasted for about 10–15 minutes, we compensated them with US$0.6 each. We paid a relatively conservative amount, lower than the average at the time (2017) because of our large sample size and to reduce the chances of the incentive affecting the overall responses due to certain takers answering the questionnaire solely for financial gains. A total number of 678 participants took part in the study. On cleaning, we were left with 659 participants for our SEM analysis. Table 2 shows the demographics of participants: 48.4% were women, while 51.6% were men – indicating that the gender distribution is almost balanced.

Demographics of participants ( n =659).

Research model

Based on the existing empirical findings in the literature and, more specifically, the theoretical social cognitive model for health promotion proposed by Bandura, 40 we formulated 10 hypotheses, as shown in Figure 2 – a follow-up SCT-based model to that of Oyibo et al. 20 In the prior model, Oyibo et al. [20] found that the perceived persuasiveness of exercise behavior models significantly influences all of the three cognitive (internal) factors of the SCT: outcome expectation, self-regulation and self-efficacy. Apart from these three internal factors, we have added an external factor (social support) to our model shown in Figure 2 with the aim of uncovering how all four SCT factors impact exercise behavior in the context of behavior modeling.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 10.1177_2055207618811555-fig2.jpg

Hypothesized social cognitive model of exercise behavior.

The first and second hypotheses (H1 and H2) were based on the findings of Rovniak et al. 24 in a study among 277 students of Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University. In their study, which modeled students’ physical activity, they found that self-efficacy strongly influenced self-regulation and outcome expectation. Thus, in our contextualized study based on exercise behavior modeling, we hypothesize that the perceived self-efficacy of the observers of the behavior will positively influence their perceived self-regulation as well as their outcome expectations. The third hypothesis (H3) was informed by the self-efficacy theory of Bandura, 35 , 40 which states that self-efficacy is the strongest (proximal) determinant of behavior in general. This theory was empirically validated by Oyibo. 30 Among university student participants resident in Canada, Oyibo 30 found that self-efficacy significantly influences their physical activity level. Based on this finding and the theory of self-efficacy, we hypothesize that, in the context of exercise behavior modeling, self-efficacy will directly influence users’ exercise behavior performance.

Furthermore, the fourth and fifth hypotheses (H4 and H5) are predicated on the social cognitive model proposed by Bandura, 40 in which the author theorized that outcome expectations positively influence goals-based self-regulation and the target behavior in health promotion. These theorized relationships were validated by the empirical study carried out by Anderson-Bill et al. 34 to model the physical activity behavior of web-health users. As a result, in our study, we hypothesize that outcome expectation will positively influence self-regulation and bodyweight exercise behavior performance. Similarly, the sixth hypothesis (H6) was informed by the theoretical social cognitive model of Bandura 40 and the empirical validation of Anderson-Bill et al. 34

Finally, the seventh and eighth hypotheses (H7 and H8) were informed by the findings of Anderson-Bill et al. 34 in the eating domain, while the ninth and tenth hypotheses (H9 and H10) were informed by their findings in the physical activity domain. In the same study, 34 the authors found that social support directly (positively) influenced self-efficacy, outcome expectation, self-regulation and physical activity (the target behavior). Hence, in our contextualized study, we hypothesize with respect to H7, H8, H9 and H10 that social support will positively influence all four social cognitive constructs as shown in Figure 2 .

Qualitative analysis

To uncover some of the main motivators and demotivators in users’ feedback with respect to the four hypothesized SCT determinants of exercise behavior (self-efficacy, self-regulation, outcome expectation and social support), we manually went through each comment to see what each participant was saying. In the discussion section, we provided a snippet of the relevant participants’ comments to support the validation of the respective hypotheses presented in Figure 2 .

In this section, we present the descriptive statistics of our data, the evaluation of our measurement models, the analysis of our structural models and the multigroup analysis (MGA).

Descriptive statistics

Table 3 shows the overall mean values for the SCT determinants and the target exercise behavior together with the standard deviations in brackets. Exercise behavior was operationalized as performance and calculated as a product of the number of repetitions of the target exercise (push-ups or squats) per day and the number of days per week. Thus, exercise behavior performance is measured in the number of reps/week. Overall, men projected more reps/week (267) than women did (142) with respect to the performance of the target behavior (push-ups/squats). Moreover, men rated their perceived self-efficacy and perceived social support higher than women did. In particular, with respect to self-efficacy, men (63.5%) had more confidence in their ability to perform bodyweight exercise than women (54.4%) did, leading to a higher exercise performance projection for men (267) than for women (142). Similarly, men (77.3%) had a stronger belief in social support from friends and family towards engaging in the target behavior than women (72.0%) did.

Rating of social cognitive constructs.

Bold values indicate men and women differ at p  < 0.05.The SCT factors are on a scale from 0 to 100%.The values in brackets represent standard deviation.

Measurement models

Our SEM analysis was carried out using the PLSPM package in R. 41 We chose this software package because it is free and has well-documented resources on how to use it to build SEM models and analyze them in R Studio (e.g. Sanchez). 42 Before carrying out the SEM analysis on the global, male and female structural models, we evaluated the respective measurement models based on the following required criteria: indicator reliability, internal consistency reliability, convergent validity and discriminant validity. 39 , 42 , 43 We briefly discuss each criterion here.

Indicator reliability

All of the indicators in the measurement models had an outer loading greater than 0.7, except for ‘[name of exercise] will strengthen my bones’, which was less than 0.7. However, given the value was not less than 0.6, it was kept in the measurement models. Moreover, the outer loading for the indicator, ‘I will make my goal public by telling others about it’, was less than 0.5, so it was dropped from self-regulation in all three models.

Internal consistency reliability

We evaluated this metric for each construct using the composite reliability criterion, Dillon-Goldstein’s rho, which was greater than 0.7.

Convergent validity

We evaluated this criterion for each construct, using the average variance extracted, which was greater than 0.5.

Discriminant validity

We assessed this criterion using the cross-loading metric of each construct on the other constructs. Our results showed that there was no indicator which loaded higher on any other construct than the construct it was meant to measure. 39 , 42 , 43

Finally, before building the respective models, we transformed the exercise behavior construct, which is based on the number of push-up/squat repetitions per week, to a normal distribution using the logarithm function (log 10 ). We did this because the original distribution was highly skewed.

Global model

Figure 3 shows the global model for the entire population sample together with the respective metrics that describe it: the goodness of fit (GOF) of the model, the coefficient of determination (R 2 ) of the endogenous constructs and the path coefficient (β). The GOF represents how well the model fits its data, while R 2 represents the amount of variance of an endogenous construct explained by the exogenous constructs. Finally, β represents the strength of the relationship between a pair of SCT constructs. In the global model, the GOF value is 50%, while the R 2 value is 11%. This indicates that social support, self-efficacy and outcome expectation combined are able to explain 11% of the variance in exercise behavior. The low explanation of the target construct by the driver constructs is an indication that the global population sample is heterogeneous and/or there are other factors, which may account for the variance of exercise behavior, that are not captured in the model.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 10.1177_2055207618811555-fig3.jpg

Global model of exercise behavior for the entire population sample.

With respect to the interrelationships among the SCT constructs, the global model shows that self-efficacy (β = 0.23, p  < 0.001), social support (β = 0.11, p  < 0.05) and outcome expectation (β = 0.23, p  < 0.01) directly (positively) influence exercise behavior. In particular, self-efficacy has the strongest direct effect on exercise behavior, while self-regulation (β =−0.07, p  = n.s.) has the weakest (no significant) effect on exercise behavior. Moreover, the strongest direct effect in the model is that between social support and self-efficacy (β = 0.52, p  < 0.001), indicating how strongly social support influences self-efficacy.

Subgroup models

We carried out a MGA to uncover the differences between the male and female groups, which make up the entire population sample. The results of our MGA indicated that there are significant differences between the male and female groups. Consequently, we built two different submodels for both genders, as shown in Figure 4 . The circular brackets represent the female submodel, while the square brackets represent the male submodel. In particular, the differences between both submodels are with respect to the three interrelationships among three specific constructs: social support, self-efficacy and exercise behavior. On one hand, the MGA showed that men and women significantly differ with respect to the relationships between social support and self-efficacy, and between self-efficacy and exercise behavior, with these direct effects being stronger for the male group than the female group. On the other hand, the MGA showed that men and women significantly differ with respect to the relationship between social support and exercise behavior, with this direct effect being stronger for the female group than for the male group. Furthermore, we found that the variance of exercise behavior remains low still (12% for the male model and 7% for the female model). Again, this is an indication of an unobserved heterogeneity unexplained by gender difference and/or an indication of other uncaptured factors in the model. (In future work, we will attempt to uncover what the unobserved heterogeneity is, including other possible factors that may increase the explanation of the target behavior.)

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 10.1177_2055207618811555-fig4.jpg

Subgroup models of exercise behavior for men and women (highlighted relationships indicate a significant gender difference ( p <0.05) based on the multigroup analysis (MGA)).

Total effect of SCT determinants on exercise behavior

We carried out a total-effect analysis to determine which of the four determinants has the strongest overall influence and weakest overall influence on exercise behavior (the target behavior). The results of our analysis ( Figure 5 ) showed that, at the global level, self-efficacy (β T  = 0.23, p  < 0.001) and social support (β T  = 0.23, p  < 0.001) have the strongest total effect on exercise behavior, followed by outcome expectation (β T  = 0.11, p  < 0.05). However, as expected, self-regulation has no significant total effect on exercise behavior (β T  =−0.07, p  < 0.05). At the subgroup level, for the male group, self-efficacy (β T  = 0.31, p  < 0.001) has the strongest total effect on exercise behavior, followed by social support (β T  = 0.20, p  < 0.001). In contrast, for the female group, social support (β T  = 0.22, p  < 0.001) has the strongest total effect on exercise behavior, followed by social support (β T  = 0.12, p  < 0.001). Furthermore, regardless of gender, outcome expectation has the third strongest total effect on exercise behavior, only that while it is completely significant for the female group (β T  = 0.11, p  < 0.05), it is marginally significant for the male group (β T  = 0.11, p  = 0.053). However, self-regulation has no significant total effect on exercise behavior for both the male and female groups, just as in the global model.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 10.1177_2055207618811555-fig5.jpg

Total effects on exercise behavior (all of the total effects are significant at p  < 0.05, except that of SR for all three models which are non-significant ( p  > 0.05) and that of OE for the male model which is marginally significant ( p  = 0.053). SE: self-efficacy; SS: social support; SR: self-regulation; OE: outcome expectation).

Mediation analysis

We carried out a mediation analysis on the respective models to determine which of the relationships are mediated by a given construct. Our results showed that there are five mediated paths in the three SEM models (see Table 4 ). At the global level, self-efficacy (variation accounted for (VAF = 0.37) and outcome expectation (VAF = 0.22) partially mediate the influence of social support on exercise behavior. In the male submodel, self-efficacy (VAF = 0.54) partially mediates the influence of social support on exercise behavior. Similarly, outcome expectation (VAF = 0.21) partially mediates the influence of social support on self-regulation. Finally, in the female model, self-efficacy (VAF 0.26) partially mediates the influence of social support on self-regulation.

Summary of the overall and gender-based findings.

SS: social support; SE: self-efficacy; SR: self-regulation; OE: outcome expectation; EB: exercise behavior.‘–’ represents path in models that does not meet the condition to test for mediation using the variation accounted for by the indirect path metric. 38 Moreover, bold values indicate partial mediation.

We have presented the results of a SEM analysis to answer our study’s research questions presented in the Research objective and design section. Table 5 summarizes all of our findings based on the verification of our 10 pre-stated hypotheses (H1–H10) and the gender-based MGA (F11–F13) based on an exploratory analysis. In particular, H1–H10 answer our first two research questions, while F11–F13 answer our third research question. At the global level, nine out of the 10 pre-stated hypotheses are validated, while, at the subgroup level, eight are validated. In the following subsections, we discuss the validation or non-validation of each of the 10 hypotheses and the three other findings based on the exploratory approach.

G: global model; F: female model; M: male model.✓: supported/validated; ×: not supported/validated.

Global validation of self-efficacy related hypotheses: H1, H2 and H3

Table 5 shows that the three hypotheses (H1, H2 and H3) related to self-efficacy are validated. This indicates that, in general, the higher the perceived self-efficacy of the observers of a modeled behavior, the higher their perceived outcome expectations and their belief in their self-regulation and the behavior performance. Moreover, based on our total-effect analysis, we found that self-efficacy (alongside social support) is the strongest determinant of bodyweight exercise behavior. This is consistent with the SCT, which holds that self-efficacy is the strongest (proximal) determinant of behavior change. According to the agentic perspective of positive psychology of Bandura, 44 humans, as agents, have the power to effect changes in their environment by their action if and only if they believe in their capability to do so. At the core of the mechanism of agency is the belief in one’s capacity (i.e. self-efficacy) to influence the circumstances and the events of one’s own life, which, according to Bandura, 44 is the ‘foundation of human motivation, well-being, and accomplishments‘ (p. 1). More specifically, Bandura argued that the self-efficacy beliefs of humans regulate their functioning through cognitive, motivational, emotional and decisional processes. He asserted that these self-efficacy beliefs, in general, influence the mindset of people (whether they will be optimistic in their thinking or pessimistic) and their outcome expectations (whether their efforts will yield favorable outcomes or not). Similarly, in our contextualized study of the SCT model of bodyweight exercise behavior performance, we found that the higher the self-efficacy of the observers of the behavior is, the higher becomes their outcome expectations (H2) and the belief in their ability to regulate themselves (H1) and perform the target behavior (push-ups/squats) ultimately (H3).

To support these quantitative findings with qualitative evidence, we provide a cross-section of the self-efficacy-related comments from participants, whose ratings of their outcome expectations, perceived self-regulation and exercise behavior performance in the survey, for the most part, reflect the level of their self-efficacy beliefs (see Table 6 ). For example, we see that P575 had a relatively high level of self-efficacy, as reflected in the qualitative comment, ‘I can do anything I set my mind to’, and the quantitative self-efficacy score of 100%. This high level of self-efficacy, in turn, might have influenced not only the participant’s outcome expectation (88%) and self-regulation (100%) but the projected exercise behavior performance (270 reps/week) as well. Compared to the overall average scores for women (see Table 3 ), we see that this participant’s scores in all four constructs are much higher. For example, for self-efficacy, outcome expectation, self-regulation and exercise behavior performance, the participant in question (P575) scored 100%, 88%, 100% and 270 reps/week, respectively, compared with the respective average scores of 54.4%, 64.9%, 72.9% and 142 reps/week for women.

Self-efficacy as a motivator of exercise behavior performance.

SE: self-efficacy; SR: self-regulation; OE: outcome expectation; EB: exercise behavior.

In contrast, for P590, who believed ‘I am kind of lazy, so I’m not sure if I’ll actually do it…’, as reflected in the low self-efficacy score (16%), compared with the respective average scores for women, her outcome expectations (56% vs. 64.9%), belief in her self-regulation (55% vs. 74.9%) and exercise behavior performance (105 reps/week vs. 142 reps/week) were correspondingly impacted (negatively) as well. In sum, the two qualitative comment-based examples (P575 and P100) concerning push-up exercises, and the other two examples (P590 and P144) concerning squat exercises (shown in Table 6 ) confirm the validation of H1, H2 and H3 in the global model (shown in Figure 3 ). In other words, they confirm that the higher (or lower) the perceived self-efficacy of the observers of a modeled behavior (push-ups or squats), the higher (or lower) their outcome expectations, their belief in their ability to regulate themselves and perform the targeted exercise behavior. Moreover, in the context of personal susceptibility to persuasive technologies, as theorized by Stibe and Larson, 13 P575 and P100 can be described as potential (‘January 1st’) users of the proposed PT intervention (fitness app), who are open and willing to change, while P590 and P144 as non-potential (‘self-contained’) users, who may not want or be willing to change.

Global validation of outcome-expectation related hypotheses: H4 and H5

The global model ( Figure 3 ) shows that the hypotheses (H4 and H5) related to outcome expectation are supported by the quantitative data as summarized in Table 5 . In other words, the higher the outcome expectations of the observers of the modeled behavior, the more likely they are to regulate themselves (H4) or perform the exercise behavior (H5). The verifications of these hypotheses are evident in the qualitative data (outcome expectation-related comments provided by participants) as well. Table 7 shows the comments of four participants (two with high outcome expectations and two with low outcome expectations) together with their SCT belief profile on self-regulation and exercise behavior performance.

Outcome expectation as a motivator of exercise behavior.

P496, for example, stated ‘push-ups will be good for me, make me stronger and make it easier for me to do regular things like lifting boxes and such’. This positive belief in the physical benefit of push-up exercise, expressed in words, is reflected in the participant’s outcome expectation belief score (78%), self-regulation belief score (95%) and projected exercise behavior performance (700 reps/week). Similarly, P187 believed ‘physical exercises make me feel better about my look, my look gives me self-confidence’. This positive belief in the social benefit of exercise, expressed in words, is reflected in the participant’s outcome expectation belief score (66%), self-regulation belief score (75%) and exercise behavior performance (910 reps/week). In contrast, P654 and P212 did not have outcome expectations as positive as those of P496 and P187. This is evident in their respective comments as well as their outcome expectation belief scores shown in Table 7 . For example, while P654 believed ‘I really can’t see it doing much other than making your arms stronger. Maybe you’d lose a tiny bit of weight. Nothing more, though’, P212 believed the squat exercise ‘may slightly help but I don’t think it would be anything life changing’. These negative outcome expectations, as reflected in the outcome expectation belief score (56% and 28%) for P654 and P212, respectively, went as far as affecting their belief in their self-regulation (40% and 25%) and performance of the target exercise behavior (120 reps/week and 20 reps/week). (These scores in brackets (and the subsequent similar ones) correspond to the respective participants aforementioned.) These scores in all three constructs (OE, SR and EB) for both participants are relatively lower than the respective overall average scores shown in Table 3 : outcome expectation (66.1%), self-regulation (71.8%) and exercise behavior (207 reps/week). Based on these results, coupled with the qualitative evidence, we conclude that our fourth and fifth hypotheses (H4 and H5), shown in Table 5 , are supported by the data. Thus, the higher (or lower) the outcome expectations of the observers of a modeled behavior (push-up or squat), the higher (or lower) their belief in their ability to regulate themselves or perform the targeted exercise behavior.

Global validation of self-regulation related hypotheses: H6

The global model ( Figure 3 ) shows that the hypothesis (H6) related to self-regulation is not supported by the quantitative data. In other words, the higher the observers’ belief in their ability to regulate themselves (i.e. set goals) may not necessarily lead to a higher exercise behavior performance (i.e. the number of repetitions of push-ups/squats per week). This finding, in light of other findings (see Figure 5 ), suggests that setting goals alone may not be enough to bring about the eventual performance of the target exercise behavior; other factors, such as self-efficacy, outcome expectations, social support, etc., may be necessary. One possible explanation for why self-regulation belief does not influence exercise behavior performance is that, in a real-life setting, people may end up not meeting their set goals given a number of reasons, which include lack of time, lack of motivation, lack of self-efficacy, lack of social support, etc. This is evident in the following comments from two of the study’s participants:

I usually have a hard time meeting my workout goals even if I set small goals. My busy life gets in the way and I just forget about doing it. (P337, push-ups)
If I have someone to help me stay on track, I might be more motivated to stick to my goals. (P387, push-ups)

P337, for example, alluded to lack of time (busy schedule) as the reason for not meeting set goals, even small goals. On the other hand, P387 alluded to lack of social support as the reason for not sticking to his/her goals. These comments reveal that setting of goals alone may not be enough motivation for someone to engage in a given exercise behavior; there have to be other drivers such as personal motivation (self-efficacy) – irrespective of busy schedules or other life challenges – and social support. 11

Global validation of social support related hypotheses: H7, H8, H9 and H10

In our global model ( Figure 3 ), we showed that both the direct relationships social support has with self-efficacy, self-regulation, outcome expectation and exercise behavior are significant. Hence, our last four hypotheses (H7, H8, H9 and H10) are supported by the global data-driven model. These hypotheses state that the higher the observers’ belief in s ocial support, the higher will be their self-efficacy, outcome expectation, self-regulation beliefs and projected exercise behavior performance. Moreover, these hypotheses are supported quantitatively (in the SEM model) and qualitatively (see participants’ response and profile shown in Table 8 ). In particular, we see that P595 and P10 believed in social support and its power to influence their exercise behavior performance, while P430 and P53 did not. These opposing beliefs in the positive benefit of social support in the performance of the target behavior went as far as influencing (directly) not only the three other determinants of exercise behavior but the target behavior as well.

Social support as a motivator of exercise behavior.

SS: social support; SE: self-efficacy; SR: self-regulation; OE: outcome expectation; EB: exercise behavior.

On one hand, P595 and P10 believed in the power of social influence as evident in their respective comments: ‘If I have someone keeping me accountable I would be more inclined to perform’ and ‘having the support of family/friends will only help me feel more motivated to exercise’. Consequently, their respective beliefs in social support (100% and 100%) influenced their self-efficacy belief (66% and 76%), outcome expectations (100% and 84%), self-regulation belief (100% and 85%) and projected exercise behavior performance (700 reps/week and 500 reps/week). These findings – which represent an overall positive effect of perceived social support on exercise behavior in our study – replicate prior findings in a self-report study among a Pakistani population. Samir et al. 11 found that lack of spouse and family support constitutes one of the main barriers to physical activity. In particular, the authors found that people from extended families are more likely to be inactive than people from nuclear active families.

On the other hand, P430 and P53 did not believe in the efficacy of social influence. This is evident in their respective comments: ‘I do not like working out with friends and family. If they pushed me I would feel pressure and most likely abandon the whole idea’, and ‘I exercise alone. Someone reminding me or telling me to pisses me off and wouldn’t encourage me at all’. Consequently, compared with the overall average scores of the respective constructs shown in Table 3 , their relatively low belief in social support (28% and 0%) influenced (decreased) their respective self-efficacy belief (32% and 66% – an exception), outcome expectation (56% and 41%), self-regulation belief (60% and 35%) and projected exercise behavior performance (75 reps/week and 150 reps/week). Based on this quantitative evidence, coupled with that in the global model (shown in Figure 3 ) and qualitative evidence (presented in Table 8 ), we conclude that H7, H8, H9 and H10, as shown in Table 5 , are supported.

Gender differences: F11, F12 and F13

Our MGA ( Figure 4 ) shows that men and women significantly differ with respect to H3, H7 and H10. These differences are summarized as F12, F11 and F13, respectively, in Table 5 . First, the influence of social support on self-efficacy is stronger for men than for women F11. This suggests that the social support received by men is more likely to influence their self-efficacy in comparison with that received by women. This is also evident in mens’ stronger belief in social support (77.3%) in their performance of bodyweight exercise compared to that of women (72.0%) as shown in Table 3 . Second, the influence of self-efficacy on the exercise behavior performance is stronger for men than for women F12. In fact, while this relationship is significant for the male group, it is not for the female group. In other words, mens’ belief in their self-efficacy influenced their projected performance of the target exercise behavior, but this is not the case for women. This finding may not be surprising, given that, generally, men have more confidence in their ability to perform physical activity, as evident in Table 3 , in which the overall average self-efficacy of men (63.5%) is significantly higher than that of women (54.4%). Finally, the influence of social support on exercise behavior performance is stronger for women than for men (F13). In fact, while this relationship is significant for women it is non-significant for men (see Figure 3 ). This suggests that, in practice, the social support women receive can directly influence their performance of the target exercise behavior, unlike men, for whom self-efficacy partially mediates the relationship between both constructs as shown in Table 4 .

General guidelines for PT design based on main findings

Based on the validated hypotheses and the qualitative comments provided by participants, we provide a number of general guidelines to inform the PT design of health interventions to encourage user engagement in bodyweight exercise behavior to improve health and wellbeing. In particular, the guidelines are based mainly on the significant determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior, which include self-efficacy, social-support and outcome-expectation. More specifically, these strategies are targeted at enhancing self-efficacy because it is the strongest proximal determinant of behavior change as evident in the SEM model shown in Figure 3 .

Guideline 1. Increase the enactive mastery experience of users (self-efficacy)

Make users be aware of their previous accomplishment of the target exercise behavior, for example, the achievement of a previous goal. Bandura 35 regarded the enactive mastery experience as the strongest source of self-efficacy. According to the self-efficacy theory, 35 each success attained by the user builds more confidence in his/her ability to repeat the achievement of the initial success. This awareness of previous success, in the face of difficulties or challenges, can serve as a booster of the user’s confidence in accomplishing the target behavior once again. The following snippets of comments from participants are examples of users being so confident in themselves due to their past performance and achievements:

I currently work out at least 3 days a week at a gym so I’m extremely confident (from experience) that I can perform the above workout. (P3, squats)
I am completely confident I can because I already do squats every day. (P5, squats)
Doing 100 push-ups has never been a problem for me. I would just need to schedule it into every day. (P617, push-ups)

Guideline 2. Allow users to collaborate with and motivate one another to perform the target behavior (social support)

Allow users to collaborate with and support one another to increase their motivation to perform the target exercise behavior. Persuasive strategies, such as peer-based or group-based cooperation, can be used as an effective social influence strategy to encourage users to perform the target behavior. Peer/social support (e.g. cooperation) is more likely to have a stronger direct effect on womens’ exercise behavior than that of men (see Figure 4 ). Competition could be used as well for certain users who are motivated by competition, for example, men and/or younger people. 45 , 46 In particular, with respect to cooperation, users feel a sense of accountability 47 and, as a result, tend to avoid disappointing their collaborative partners once they have committed themselves. The following are examples of participants’ comments that attest to the feeling of accountability in particular and the efficacy of cooperation and competition strategies in general:

Having someone relying on me to do an activity with them is the number one way for me to commit to actually doing it. Not letting someone down is a very strong motivator. (P371, push-ups)
I think the support system would make a big difference in motivation. It gives accountability, as well as competition. (P9, squats)
Working with a friend motivates you to complete the task and has an added level of competition. I am less likely to skip my workout if I have a friend present who is also doing it. (P51, squats)
I am easily motivated by others rather than by myself. If my family, friends or coworkers did the exercises with me, I would feel the pressure to do them as well. I am never one to turn down a challenge, so creating a game out of it or having a competition with it. (P68, squats)
It’s always easier when you have a friend around to encourage you, push you and share your pain. Competition is also encouraging. (P 556, push-ups)

Guideline 3. Model the behavior so that users can observe its performance and visualize its outcomes (outcome-expectation)

Provide users with a means to observe the performance of the behavior and its outcomes. One way to achieve this is the use of behavior modeling (see Figure 1 ) or simulation strategy, which models the causes and effects of the performance of the behavior. Behavior modeling is regarded as the second strongest source of self-efficacy. 48 According to Bandura, 35 when the user observes a peer or role model successfully perform the target behavior, he/she will feel more confident in him/herself to perform it as well. The following comments from participants on the visual design of the behavior models, shown in Figure 1 , attest to the potential effectiveness of behavior modeling as a persuasive strategy for motivating the performance of a target exercise behavior:

Exercise in the mentioned video looks like it makes a person stronger and fit. (P284, man, push-ups)
Having this app do exercise with you makes me feel more inclined to exercise. (P657, push-ups)
Watching this gives me the impression to do this. (P574, push-ups)

Summary and contributions

In this section, we provide the summary of our main findings, which double as our contributions to the existing body of knowledge. In particular, we contribute to knowledge in the field of theory-informed PT health interventions. Our main findings and contributions can be summarized as follows:

  • We validated the SCT model in the context of behavior modeling of bodyweight exercise behavior in a fitness app prototype aimed at motivating behavior change.
  • We showed that self-efficacy and social support, followed by outcome expectation, are the strongest determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior.
  • We showed that self-regulation (i.e. goal-setting), when self-efficacy, social support and outcome expectation are controlled for, does not have a significant influence on bodyweight exercise behavior.
  • We showed that the direct effect of social support on bodyweight exercise behavior is significantly stronger for women than for men.
  • We showed that the direct effect of social support on self-efficacy and the direct effect of self-efficacy on bodyweight exercise behavior are significantly stronger for men than for women.
  • We provided a set of general guidelines, based on the significant SCT determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior, for the design of persuasive apps aimed at motivating behavior change in the fitness domain.

Limitations and future work

Our study has a number of limitations. The first and foremost limitation of the study is that it is based on a hypothetical fitness app – and not an actual fitness app modeling exercise behaviors. Second, our findings are based on the impact of perceived belief (SCT) constructs (such as self-efficacy) on the performance of exercise behavior. For these reasons, our findings, which are based on self-report, may not generalize to a real-life application setting in which the study participants would have to answer questions on the SCT factors and subsequently use the health app over a period of time, with their exercise performance and activities being tracked. For example, perceived self-regulation, which has a non-significant impact on participants’ projected exercise performance, may turn out to have a significant impact on the latter in a real-life fitness application. Thus, to bridge this gap in our current study, we recommend that the replication of our validated SEM model be verified using data gathered from a real-life health application. The third limitation of our study is that it focused specifically on participants resident in North America, who were mostly Canadian and American citizens. This may threaten the generalizability of our findings to users from other continents, countries and cultures. Thus, in future research efforts in the area of PTs for promoting exercise behavior, we recommend that our study be conducted among other demographics to uncover the generalizability of our findings.

Behavior modeling is one of the main behavior change techniques through which humans observe the actions and consequences of the behaviors of other people, and ultimately acquire the necessary knowledge and skills to engage in the modeled behavior. In this paper, using SCT as a theoretical framework of behavior change, we investigated, in the context of behavior modeling, which of the SCT (belief) constructs are the strongest determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior performance on the home front. To uncover the determinants of exercise behavior performance, we carried out an empirical study among 659 participants resident in the USA and Canada, using behavior models demonstrating push-up and squat exercises as a case study. The results of our SEM analysis showed that perceived self-efficacy and perceived social support are the strongest determinants of bodyweight exercise behavior, followed by outcome expectation. Moreover, in our SEM model, perceived self-regulation turns out to have a non-significant influence on bodyweight exercise behavior. Moreover, our results showed that the direct and total effect of self-efficacy on bodyweight exercise behavior is stronger for men than for women, while the direct and total effect of social support on bodyweight exercise behavior is stronger for women than for men. Finally, based on the significant SCT determinants, we recommend a set of design guidelines to inform the implementation of persuasive health apps to drive behavior change in the fitness domain.

Contributorship

The first author designed and conducted the study. All authors contributed in preparing the paper.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

The study complied with the research ethics guidelines provided by the University of Saskatchewan. The ethics approval number is BEH-17362.

The authors would like to thank the Canadian Federal Government and the Saskatchewan Government for funding this research. The third author received the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grant, while the first and second authors received the Saskatchewan Innovation and Opportunity Scholarship.

Not applicable.

Peer review

This manuscript was reviewed by two reviewers. The authors have elected for these individuals to remain anonymous.

IMAGES

  1. Social Cognitive Theory PowerPoint Presentation Slides

    case study social cognitive theory

  2. (PDF) Case study: Using social cognitive theory and social support

    case study social cognitive theory

  3. Illustration of Social Cognitive Theory 3 An illustration of Social

    case study social cognitive theory

  4. Conceptual Framework of the Study. Note. SCT: Social cognitive theory

    case study social cognitive theory

  5. Model of Social Cognitive Theory Stock Illustration

    case study social cognitive theory

  6. PPT

    case study social cognitive theory

VIDEO

  1. Social Cognitive Theory

  2. Schoolwork Studies: Social Cognitive Theory

  3. Notes CDP Social Cognitive Theory Albert Bandura Video-11 Pstet Child Development and Pedagogy

  4. Social Cognitive Theory

  5. Social cognitive theory by Albert Bandura in Urdu

  6. Social Cognitive Theory Discussion

COMMENTS

  1. A Systematic Review Exploring the Social Cognitive Theory of Self-Regulation as a Framework for Chronic Health Condition Interventions

    The social cognitive theory of self-regulation proposes that three main components of the theory, self-monitoring, self-judgement, and self-evaluation, contribute to self-regulation, and influence successful behaviour change. ... case studies, theses, reviews, summaries, commentaries, editorials, letters to the editor, or study protocols ...

  2. Albert Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory

    Social cognitive theory emphasizes the learning that occurs within a social context. In this view, people are active agents who can both influence and are influenced by their environment. The theory was founded most prominently by Albert Bandura, who is also known for his work on observational learning, self-efficacy, and reciprocal determinism.

  3. (PDF) Case study: Using social cognitive theory and social support

    The program was based on Ajzen's theory of planned behavior (2005), Bandura's social-cognitive theory (1997) and cognitive-behavior theory of behavioral change (Meichenbaum, 1993).

  4. The social‐cognitive clinician: On the implications of social cognitive

    We review Bandura's contributions to cognitive-behavioural theory, research and practice. His basic research on the causal role of cognitive processes in social learning was a major factor in the emergence of cognitive-behavioural therapies in the 1970s.His investigations on observational learning and self-efficacy beliefs led to the development of guided mastery therapy, a specific cognitive ...

  5. Theory and Application in Personality Science: The Case of Social

    Three central themes in social-cognitive conceptions are identified: (a) a focus on individuals rather than on summaries of individual differences in the population, (b) attention to causal mechanisms underlying action and experience and (c) the study of persons in context, including attention to psychological processes through which people ...

  6. Clinical Application of Social Cognitive Theory: A Novel Personality

    We present a novel personality assessment method that applies social cognitive personality theory, and more specifically, the Knowledge and Appraisal Personality Architecture model (KAPA; Cervone, 2004; 2021; see Scott & Cervone, 2016).Our assessment method generates descriptions of how personality structures, including temperament, beliefs, goals, and evaluative standards, are activated in ...

  7. (PDF) Clinical Application of Social Cognitive Theory: A Novel

    Clinical Application of Social Cognitive Theory: A Novel Personality Assessment Procedure and a Case Study of Personality Coherence July 2021 European Journal of Personality 36(2):089020702110283

  8. 3

    Summary. Social cognitive theory focuses on the reciprocal interaction of the person, environment, and behavior and provides a description of the ways in which individuals initiate and maintain behaviors, taking into consideration their social environment. The main operative constructs in the theory are outcome expectancies and self-efficacy.

  9. Theory and Application in Personality Science: The Case of Social

    Theory and Application in Personality Science: The Case of Social-cognitive Theory. August 2023. Psychology and Developing Societies 35 (2) DOI: 10.1177/09713336231178366. Authors: Daniel Cervone ...

  10. Case study: using social cognitive theory and social support coping

    In this case study, social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986, 2004) and social support as a way of coping (Vitaliano et al., 1985) have been selected to overcome barriers created by low levels of self-confidence and perceived lack of support in the context of breastfeeding. Thus, the application of the two theories in this case study is designed ...

  11. Social Cognitive Theory

    Social Cognitive Theory is a social psychological theory that aims to reveal how individuals' internal knowledge structure and belief system explain and give meaning to social objects and their interrelationships. According to Gestalt psychology, the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. Therefore, an understanding of the whole requires ...

  12. Social Cognitive Theory: Definition and Examples

    Updated on January 20, 2019. Social cognitive theory is a learning theory developed by the renowned Stanford psychology professor Albert Bandura. The theory provides a framework for understanding how people actively shape and are shaped by their environment. In particular, the theory details the processes of observational learning and modeling ...

  13. Social cognition theories and behavior change in COVID-19: A conceptual

    These studies have progressed knowledge on the social cognition correlates of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, and we look to future research that further elicits specific beliefs concerning COVID-19 prevention and examines their effects on preventive behaviors. 2.3.3. Study design and inferences.

  14. Social Cognitive Personality Assessment: A Case Conceptualization

    Social-cognitive theory has had minimal influence on personality assessment and case conceptualization practices despite significant recent developments in theoretically driven assessment principles and strategies ... In our case study, S.L. benefitted from realizing that both her self-as-inferior/ruined schemata, as well as her inhibited ...

  15. Applying social cognitive theory to predict physical activity and

    The present study results show that social cognitive theory domains impact the physical activity and dietary behaviors of patients with diabetes undergoing medication. The re- sults indicate that patients who had set goals and con- stantly monitored themselves had the self-efficacy to en- gage in behaviors to manage their diabetes by including ...

  16. Social Cognitive Theory

    Social cognitive theory is a general theory that stresses learning from the social environment. From its early focus on observational learning through modeling, social cognitive theory has expanded in scope to address such processes as motivation and self-regulation. Bandura's social cognitive theory postulates reciprocal interactions among ...

  17. Using Social Cognitive Theory to Predict Medication Compliance Behavior

    The social cognitive theory (SCT) is a theory of human behavior based on the assumption that expectations, thoughts, and beliefs influence's ones behavior and is shaped by the individuals' social environment. 18,19 This approach has not been tested for compliance behavior in depression patients. In this study, the SCT will be used to ...

  18. Social cognitive theory and physical activity ...

    Albert Bandura's (1986, 2001) social cognitive theory (SCT) represents one of the most studied theories in the field of psychology, with applications in contexts as diverse as education, healthcare, rehabilitation, the legal system, business, and indeed sport and exercise. It is positioned as a theory of human behavior with integrative principles of broad applicability (Bandura, 1998).

  19. Social Cognitive Theory and Health Care: Analysis and Evaluation

    Social Cognitive Theory showed that even though is a non-disciplinary theory of health sciences, the clarity and simplicity of its content facilitates its use in understanding and addressing ...

  20. Social Cognitive Theory

    3 Social Cognitive Theory At the end of this chapter, you will be able to: ... the University of Iowa was central to psychological study, especially in the area of social learning theory. By 1952, Bandura completed his Master's and Ph.D. in clinical psychology. ... in the case of a student, the instructions the teacher provides help students ...

  21. Social cognitive determinants of exercise behavior in the context of

    Social cognitive theory. SCT is a behavior theory of human motivation and action. It is an offshoot of the SLT 21 proposed by Bandura 22 to explain the various internal and external processes (cognitive, vicarious, self-reflective and self-regulatory) that come into play in human psychosocial functioning. It is organized within a causation framework known as the triadic reciprocal determinism ...

  22. Social cognitive theory

    Social cognitive theory is a psychological perspective that examines how people learn from observing and interacting with others. This webpage from IB Psych Matters, a website that provides useful and updated resources for IB psychology students, introduces the main concepts and principles of social cognitive theory, such as self-efficacy, reciprocal determinism, and observational learning ...

  23. (PDF) Social Cognitive Theory

    Abstract. In conceptualizations presented in social cognitive theory (SCT), humans are not passive objects shaped and shepherded by contingent consequences of an environment. People are agentic ...