ScienceDaily

Educational Psychology News

Top headlines, latest headlines.

  • Losing Your Keys Is Normal
  • Educational Intervention That Works
  • Mice Think Like Babies
  • Spinal Cord Nerve Cells Learn and Remember
  • Most Teens Worry How Sick Days Impact Grades?
  • Effective Teachers: Range of Student Abilities
  • Early Intervention in Epilepsy Is Important
  • Neighboring Synapses Shape Learning, Memory
  • Antipsychotics Use in Pregnancy
  • Preventing Post-COVID 'Brain Fog'

Earlier Headlines

Wednesday, april 24, 2024.

  • Computer Game in School Made Students Better at Detecting Fake News

Monday, April 22, 2024

  • Japan's Premodern Concept of Nature at Root of Distinctive Mindset in Early Childhood Education

Wednesday, April 17, 2024

  • Gender Stereotypes in Schools Impact on Girls and Boys With Mental Health Difficulties, Study Finds

Monday, April 15, 2024

  • School Suspensions and Exclusions Put Vulnerable Children at Risk

Thursday, March 28, 2024

  • Is It the School, or the Students?

Monday, March 18, 2024

  • Study Estimates Nearly 70 Percent of Children Under Six in Chicago May Be Exposed to Lead-Contaminated Tap Water

Thursday, March 14, 2024

  • Study of Long-Term Student Engagement Challenges 'one Great Teacher' Narrative of Education

Wednesday, March 13, 2024

  • Innovative Approaches for Addressing Difficult Topics in K-12 Schools
  • Multiple Air Pollutants Linked to Asthma Symptoms in Children
  • Explicit Socioemotional Learning Can Have a Key Role in PE Lessons, Study Says

Tuesday, March 12, 2024

  • Study: Best Way to Memorize Stuff? It Depends ...

Monday, March 11, 2024

  • Happiness Can Be Learnt, but You Have to Work at It
  • No, an Anti-Racist Program in Schools Didn't Stress out Kids, Study Finds

Friday, March 8, 2024

  • Small Class Sizes Not Better for Pupils' Grades or Resilience, Says Study

Wednesday, March 6, 2024

  • Study Reveals Unexpected Literacy in Autistic People Who Cannot Speak

Friday, March 1, 2024

  • Link Between Adversity, Psychiatric and Cognitive Decline
  • Early Vocabulary Size Is Genetically Linked to ADHD, Literacy, and Cognition

Thursday, February 29, 2024

  • Scientists Identify New 'regulatory' Function of Learning and Memory Gene Common to All Mammalian Brain Cells

Tuesday, February 27, 2024

  • New Study Links Placental Oxygen Levels to Fetal Brain Development
  • Maths: Smart Learning Software Helps Children During Lockdowns -- And Beyond
  • Learning and Memory Problems in Down Syndrome Linked to Alterations in Genome's 'dark Matter'

Friday, February 23, 2024

  • School Focus on Grades, Test Scores Linked to Violence Against Teachers

Wednesday, February 21, 2024

  • Sleep Improves Ability to Recall Complex Events
  • Teachers' Growth Mindset Appears More Important Than Warmth

Tuesday, February 20, 2024

  • Study Finds Students, Designers Have Different Perceptions of Masculine, Feminine Traits of Classrooms
  • Blocking Key Protein May Halt Progression of Alzheimer's Disease

Friday, February 16, 2024

  • Games in the Classroom and the Boardroom: How 'serious Games' Are Helping Us Learn

Thursday, February 15, 2024

  • The Brain Is 'programmed' For Learning from People We Like

Tuesday, February 13, 2024

  • Oxytocin: The Love Hormone That Holds the Key to Better Memory

Monday, February 12, 2024

  • Children's Positive Attitude Towards Mathematics Fades During the Early School Years

Wednesday, February 7, 2024

  • How Teachers Make Ethical Judgments When Using AI in the Classroom

Thursday, February 1, 2024

  • Hiring the Most Qualified Candidate Might Be Unfair

Tuesday, January 30, 2024

  • Citizen Scientists Contribute to Motor Learning Research

Monday, January 22, 2024

  • Navigating the 'big Little Leap' To Kindergarten

Thursday, January 18, 2024

  • Physical Exercise Boosts Motor Learning -- And Remembering What One Has Learned

Wednesday, January 17, 2024

  • Certain Personality Traits Linked to College Students' Sense of Belonging

Saturday, January 13, 2024

  • Let Me Check My Phone Again

Wednesday, January 10, 2024

  • Neuropsychological Effects of Rapid-Acting Antidepressants May Explain Their Clinical Benefits

Monday, December 18, 2023

  • Parents' Top Resolutions: More Patience, Less Time on Phones
  • AI's Memory-Forming Mechanism Found to Be Strikingly Similar to That of the Brain
  • Memory Research: Breathing in Sleep Impacts Memory Processes

Wednesday, December 13, 2023

  • Yoga Nidra Might Be a Path to Better Sleep and Improved Memory
  • Machine Learning Sees Into the Future to Prevent Sight Loss in Humans

Thursday, December 7, 2023

  • New HS Curriculum Teaches Color Chemistry and AI Simultaneously

Friday, December 1, 2023

  • Human Behavior Guided by Fast Changes in Dopamine Levels
  • Why Reading Nursery Rhymes and Singing to Babies May Help Them to Learn Language
  • Child-Centric Approach: Blueprint to Improve Communities

Thursday, November 30, 2023

  • Researchers Discover New Classes of RNA for Learning and Memory

Wednesday, November 29, 2023

  • Brittle Stars Can Learn Just Fine -- Even Without a Brain
  • AI May Aid in Diagnosing Adolescents With ADHD

Monday, November 27, 2023

  • Extra Practice Blending Letter Sounds Helps Struggling Readers

Tuesday, November 21, 2023

  • How Do We Learn? Neuroscientists Pinpoint How Memories Are Likely to Be Stored in the Brain

Tuesday, November 14, 2023

  • How Teachers Would Handle Student Violence Against Educators

Tuesday, November 7, 2023

  • When Dads Are Feeling a Bit Depressed or Anxious, How Do Kids Fare?

Friday, November 3, 2023

  • Opioid Disorder Treatment: First Three Weeks Forecast Success

Monday, October 30, 2023

  • High Engagement, High Return: The Secret to Student Success

Wednesday, October 25, 2023

  • Can AI Grasp Related Concepts After Learning Only One?
  • A Sustainable Future Is Based on a Learning Society

Tuesday, October 24, 2023

  • What an Animated Taco Reveals About Curiosity and Patience

Monday, October 23, 2023

  • People Who Communicate More, Show Expertise Are More Likely to Be Seen as Essential Team Members

Friday, October 20, 2023

  • Does Suspending Kids from School Harm Their Grades and Health?

Thursday, October 19, 2023

  • Grouping English Learners in Classrooms Yields No Benefit in Reading Development, New Study Finds

Tuesday, October 17, 2023

  • Study Examines Role of Working Memory, Cognitive Functions in English Learners Learning to Write

Thursday, October 12, 2023

  • Traumatic Memories Can Rewire the Brain
  • Study Reveals Shyness Could Impact Young Children's Performance on Language Tests

Thursday, October 5, 2023

  • Awe-Inspiring Science Can Have a Positive Effect on Mental Wellbeing

Thursday, September 28, 2023

  • Protein P53 Regulates Learning, Memory, Sociability in Mice

Wednesday, September 27, 2023

  • Origin of Cultural Learning: Babies Imitate Because They Are Imitated

Friday, September 22, 2023

  • Jellyfish, With No Central Brain, Shown to Learn from Past Experience
  • Trigonelline Derived from Coffee Improves Cognitive Functions in Mice

Wednesday, September 20, 2023

  • Young Children Do Better at School If Their Dads Read and Play With Them

Tuesday, September 19, 2023

  • Breaking in the Black Box of Pedagogical Authority
  • Dopamine Regulates How Quickly and Accurately Decisions Are Made
  • Assessing Unintended Consequences in AI-Based Neurosurgical Training

Monday, September 18, 2023

  • New Research Highlights Importance of Equity in Education

Thursday, September 14, 2023

  • Vocal Learning Linked to Problem Solving Skills and Brain Size

Wednesday, September 13, 2023

  • Potential New Approach to PTSD Treatment

Tuesday, September 12, 2023

  • In the Age of ChatGPT, What's It Like to Be Accused of Cheating?

Thursday, August 31, 2023

  • Electrical Noise Stimulation Applied to the Brain Could Be Key to Boosting Math Learning

Wednesday, August 30, 2023

  • Discoveries on Memory Mechanisms Could Unlock New Therapies for Alzheimer's and Other Brain Diseases
  • Surprising Study Results: Students Are Bored During Exams
  • Researcher Combats Bullying of Students With Disabilities

Wednesday, August 23, 2023

  • Knowledge of Building Blocks of Words Plays an Important Role When Deaf Children Learn to Read, Analysis Shows

Tuesday, August 22, 2023

  • Want to Increase Resiliency in Kids? Teach Creativity

Thursday, August 17, 2023

  • A Healthy Diet, Reading, and Doing Sports Promote Reasoning Skills in Children
  • Anti-Obesity Drug Improves Associative Learning in People With Obesity

Monday, August 14, 2023

  • Can AI Help Hospitals Spot Patients in Need of Extra Non-Medical Assistance?

Thursday, August 10, 2023

  • Perils of Not Being Attractive or Athletic in Middle School

Wednesday, August 9, 2023

  • How Sure Is Sure? Incorporating Human Error Into Machine Learning
  • Dissecting the Anatomy of a 'superheroic' Science Class
  • 'Ebb and Flow' Brain Mechanism That Drives Learning Identified

Tuesday, August 8, 2023

  • Poor Time Management Causes Poor Sleep for College Students

Monday, August 7, 2023

  • Memory, Forgetting, and Social Learning
  • Mathematical Theory Predicts Self-Organized Learning in Real Neurons

Thursday, August 3, 2023

  • Social Media Algorithms Exploit How Humans Learn from Their Peers

Monday, July 31, 2023

  • Researchers Find Little Evidence of Cheating With Online, Unsupervised Exams
  • GPT-3 Can Reason About as Well as a College Student, Psychologists Report

Thursday, July 27, 2023

  • Physical Activity Can Promote Learning and Wellbeing at Secondary School

Tuesday, July 25, 2023

  • Scientists May Have Discovered Mechanism Behind Cognitive Decline in Aging

Saturday, July 22, 2023

  • Bodybuilding Supplement May Help Stave Off Alzheimer's
  • LATEST NEWS
  • Top Science
  • Top Physical/Tech
  • Top Environment
  • Top Society/Education
  • Health & Medicine
  • Mind & Brain
  • Disorders and Syndromes
  • ADD and ADHD
  • Alzheimer's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Borderline Personality Disorder
  • Brain Injury
  • Hearing Impairment
  • Huntington's Disease
  • Mad Cow Disease
  • Multiple Sclerosis
  • Obstructive Sleep Apnea
  • Parkinson's
  • Schizophrenia
  • Sleep Disorders
  • Education & Learning
  • Brain-Computer Interfaces
  • Educational Psychology
  • Infant and Preschool Learning
  • Intelligence
  • K-12 Education
  • Language Acquisition
  • Learning Disorders
  • Illegal Drugs
  • Crystal Meth
  • Psychedelic Drugs
  • Living Well
  • Anger Management
  • Child Development
  • Consumer Behavior
  • Dieting and Weight Control
  • Gender Difference
  • Nutrition Research
  • Racial Issues
  • Relationships
  • Spirituality
  • Mental Health
  • Eating Disorders
  • Smoking Addiction
  • Neuroscience
  • Child Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Space & Time
  • Matter & Energy
  • Computers & Math
  • Plants & Animals
  • Earth & Climate
  • Fossils & Ruins
  • Science & Society
  • Business & Industry

Strange & Offbeat

  • Extreme Impacts: Metals Stronger When Heated
  • Rare Earth Element's Secrets Exposed
  • Origin of Sun's Magnetic Field
  • Measuring Spin of Supermassive Black Hole
  • 'Cell Cannibalism' Across Tree of Life
  • Very Low Emission Concrete at Scale
  • Kangaroos: Fearing Human 'Super Predator'
  • Stopping Flu Before It Takes Hold
  • Cosmic Rays Illuminate the Past
  • Star Suddenly Vanish from the Night Sky

Trending Topics

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • My Account Login
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 09 May 2024

An exploration into the causal relationships between educational attainment, intelligence, and wellbeing: an observational and two-sample Mendelian randomisation study

  • J. M. Armitage 1 ,
  • R. E. Wootton 2 , 3 ,
  • O. S. P. Davis 4 &
  • C. M. A. Haworth 2  

npj Mental Health Research volume  3 , Article number:  23 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

364 Accesses

30 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Epidemiology
  • Genetics research

Educational attainment is associated with a range of positive outcomes, yet its impact on wellbeing is unclear, and complicated by high correlations with intelligence. We use genetic and observational data to investigate for the first time, whether educational attainment and intelligence are causally and independently related to wellbeing. Results from our multivariable Mendelian randomisation demonstrated a positive causal impact of a genetic predisposition to higher educational attainment on wellbeing that remained after accounting for intelligence, and a negative impact of intelligence that was independent of educational attainment. Observational analyses suggested that these associations may be subject to sex differences, with benefits to wellbeing greater for females who attend higher education compared to males. For intelligence, males scoring more highly on measures related to happiness were those with lower intelligence. Our findings demonstrate a unique benefit for wellbeing of staying in school, over and above improving cognitive abilities, with benefits likely to be greater for females compared to males.

Similar content being viewed by others

research articles in educational psychology

Investigating the genetic architecture of noncognitive skills using GWAS-by-subtraction

research articles in educational psychology

Estimating effects of parents’ cognitive and non-cognitive skills on offspring education using polygenic scores

research articles in educational psychology

Evidence for specificity of polygenic contributions to attainment in English, maths and science during adolescence

Introduction.

In most societies, education provides young people with the knowledge, skills, and socialisation necessary to prepare for adult life. The number of years spent in schooling can therefore be an important determinant of later outcomes and functioning, as evidenced by greater occupational status and income, marriage, and health 1 , 2 . The extent to which some of these relationships are causal, however, remains less clear. Educational attainment has been shown to causally impact smoking, sedentary behaviours, and Body Mass Index 3 , as well as the risk of suicide attempts, insomnia, and major depressive disorder 4 . Yet also fundamental to health and success is wellbeing 5 , but the causal impact of educational attainment on wellbeing remains unexplored.

Wellbeing is broadly defined as relating to feelings of satisfaction and happiness 6 . Observational studies investigating the impact of educational attainment on wellbeing have produced mixed results, with evidence to suggest both direct 7 and indirect effects 2 , as well as both positive and negative influences 8 . Indirect effects of education refer to those that occur via mechanisms other than education itself, such as through income, employment, marriage, children, or health 2 . Positive indirect influences of educational attainment on wellbeing have been noted largely through income, with males and females experiencing benefits of education through increased earnings 2 . Some sex differences have been noted for other indirect paths, like employment, whereby the wellbeing of educated males but not females is heightened through being employed 2 . When these indirect paths are not accounted for, associations between educational attainment and wellbeing have been shown to be negative 2 , suggesting that education exerts its benefits through many different channels. Most of the findings to date however, are based on samples from Australia, with just one study to date investigating associations in a UK sample 9 . This study found little effect of educational attainment on happiness, and little impact of a reform that raised the school leaving age. No study has jointly considered the role of intelligence and schooling on overall wellbeing.

Intelligence is often used to refer to the many facets of cognitive functioning, including memory and learning, processing speed, as well as abstract, verbal, and spatial reasoning 10 . These abilities are all interrelated and highly correlated with educational attainment 11 , yet observational findings have suggested associations with wellbeing may differ to those found for educational attainment. In particular, associations between intelligence and wellbeing are often positive, but switch to negative after accounting for other correlated factors like income and parental education 12 , 13 . This has been suggested to reflect the greater expectations that come with being highly intelligent and a higher earner 12 . The correlational nature of these studies, however, does not permit causal inferences for either the direct or indirect effects.

Determining whether associations are causal or driven by unobserved or imprecisely measured confounders is crucial to establishing true and unbiased effects. Mendelian randomisation (MR) is a study design that uses summary-level genetic data to assess potentially causal relationships 14 . The methods of MR enable control over both confounding and reverse causality, and can be extended to multivariable MR when teasing apart the combined and independent effects of highly correlated variables, like educational attainment and intelligence.

So far, MR studies have revealed that despite their high correlation, intelligence and educational attainment exert independent causal effects on some health and economic outcomes 3 , 11 . Extending such findings to wellbeing could therefore help to inform best practice for maximising optimal functioning. In particular, if associations between educational attainment and wellbeing are largely accounted for by intelligence, policy makers would benefit from focusing less on keeping students in higher education, and more on improving cognitive abilities. If, however, educational attainment exerts a direct positive impact on wellbeing, policy makers would benefit from extending requirements to remain in further education.

Few studies to date have studied causal associations with wellbeing 15 , and even fewer have made use of the latest genetic instrument for wellbeing 16 . This instrument combines four wellbeing related traits (life satisfaction, positive affect, neuroticism, and depressive symptoms), which are referred to collectively as the wellbeing spectrum. This phenotype has been associated with more genetic signals than previous genetic analyses based on positive affect and life satisfaction alone 17 . The first part of this study therefore makes use of this instrument for wellbeing in univariable and multivariable MR to test for the first time, whether educational attainment and intelligence are causally and independently related to wellbeing. Bidirectional associations are also explored as findings have shown that wellbeing not only results from successful outcomes, but it also precedes them 18 . Understanding whether associations work both ways could therefore highlight important paths to improving overall functioning.

One drawback of using the MR design is that estimates are not time bound, meaning implications for intervention may be less clear. The second part of this study therefore supplements genetic findings using longitudinal observational data. The aim is to understand the impact of educational attainment and intelligence on wellbeing in emerging adulthood, a critical life stage for establishing identity and adult mental health. Such analyses aimed to also further scrutinise the relationship between educational attainment and wellbeing to clarify possible sex differences 8 , 19 , non-linear trends 20 , and moderating effects of intelligence.

Univariable MR testing causal associations between educational attainment and intelligence

Prior to investigating effects on wellbeing, univariable MR was first used to confirm the bidirectional relationship between educational attainment and intelligence 11 . Analyses revealed strong causal effects of educational attainment on intelligence, and vice versa (Supplementary Table 1 ). Effect sizes were twofold greater for educational attainment on intelligence, aligning with previous findings 11 . There was also strong evidence of heterogeneity in the causal estimates for both directions, also replicating previous findings.

Univariable MR testing causal associations between educational attainment and wellbeing

Univariable MR analyses exploring total causal effects of educational attainment on wellbeing provided evidence of a small positive impact (Fig. 1 ). For every standard deviation (SD) increase in years of schooling, which equates to 3.6 years of schooling, there was a 0.057 (95% CI = 0.042, 0.074) increase in wellbeing, as assessed using the wellbeing spectrum. There was also evidence of a causal impact of wellbeing on educational attainment. Analyses revealed that a SD increase in wellbeing predicted a 0.206 (95% CI = 0.071, 0.341) increase in the number of years schooling (see Supplementary Table 2 ).

figure 1

The main analysis is the inverse variance weighted estimate. The MR-Egger, Weighted median, and weighted mode represent sensitivity analyses.

Neither of these findings replicated using MR-Egger (see Fig. 1 ), which as explained in the methods, is unlikely to be a result of directional pleiotropy as the MR-Egger intercept did not differ from zero. The funnel plots also provided evidence of balanced pleiotropy and there was no evidence in the forest plots to suggest that associations were strongly driven by one single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) (see Supplementary Figs. 1 and 2 ). Steiger filtering revealed that all educational attainment SNPs were more associated with educational attainment than wellbeing, and sensitivity analyses removing 4 wellbeing SNPs that explained more of the variance in educational attainment revealed consistent results (see Supplementary Table 3 ), suggesting minimal bias from reverse causation. Instead, given the large confidence intervals and the low regression dilution statistic (See Supplementary Table 4 ), it is likely that MR-Egger results can be accounted for by measurement error.

Analyses predicting subjective happiness and life satisfaction using the trait-specific estimates from the model-averaging GWAMA 16 revealed largely consistent findings, with effect sizes most similar to the full wellbeing spectrum for analyses predicting life satisfaction (see Supplementary Tables 5 to 6). As anticipated, results for neuroticism and depression produced associations in the opposite direction (Supplementary Tables 7 and 8 ).

Univariable MR testing causal associations between intelligence and wellbeing

Univariable MR analyses investigating associations between intelligence and wellbeing revealed no causal effects of intelligence on wellbeing, but evidence of a causal impact of wellbeing on intelligence (Fig. 1 ). Effect sizes were similar to those found for educational attainment, with a SD increase in wellbeing predictive of a 0.199 (95% CI = 0.014, 0.390) increase in intelligence (Supplementary Table 2 ). This did not replicate using MR-Egger, but was consistent across other sensitivity analyses. As per analyses on educational attainment, findings also provided no evidence of bias due to directional horizontal pleiotropy (see Supplementary Fig. 3 ). Analyses conducted after removing 11 wellbeing SNPs following Steiger filtering also revealed largely consistent results (see Supplementary Table 3 ).

Multivariable MR

Results from the multivariable MR analysis revealed independent causal effects of both educational attainment and intelligence on wellbeing (see Fig. 2 ), however, findings were in the opposite direction to one another. For educational attainment, a one SD increase in years of schooling (3.6 years) predicted a 0.103 (95% CI = 0.05, 0.16) increase in wellbeing, controlling for the effects of intelligence, while intelligence predicted a 0.04 (95% CI = −0.08, −0.01) decrease in wellbeing, controlling for years of schooling. These findings were both larger than those found in the univariable models and were generated despite relatively weak instruments (F-statistic = 7.94 for intelligence and F-statistic = 7.23 for educational attainment). These F-statistics are lower than those in the univariable analyses due to estimating the impact of the SNPs on one exposure, conditioning on the other 21 .

figure 2

This figure shows that years of schooling has positive independent (multivariable) and total (univariable) causal effects on wellbeing. In contrast, intelligence has negative independent (multivariable) but not total (univariable) causal effects on wellbeing.

Findings from the multivariable MR-Egger analyses produced the same pattern of results as above for both exposures (Table 1 ), and the MR-Egger intercept provided no evidence of directional pleiotropy. All univariable and multivariable MR findings also remained after adjustment for multiple testing using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure 22 . Raw p-values are therefore reported to ensure consistency with the wider MR literature 23 .

To further test the bi-directional relationship between educational attainment and wellbeing, we performed an additional multivariable MR analysis in which we investigated intelligence and well-being on years of schooling. Findings revealed that wellbeing was independently associated with years of schooling after accounting for intelligence, predicting a 0.193 (95% CI = 0.07, 0.31) increase in years of schooling, and intelligence was independently associated with years of schooling after accounting for wellbeing, predicting a 0.44 (95% CI = 0.40, 0.48) increase in years of schooling.

Observational findings

Descriptives.

Observational analyses were conducted using the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC 24 ). Among participants with data on educational attainment, intelligence, and wellbeing, approximately 66.7% had a university degree. Individuals who had a university degree scored significantly higher on the intelligence test at 8 years old (mean = 112.21, SD = 14.75, range = 62−148) compared to individuals without a university degree (mean = 99.07, SD = 14.93, range = 45−138), according to a Welch two sample t test, t(1879) = −22.2, p  < 0.001.

Subjective happiness scores in the samples averaged 4.89 (range = 1 to 7), while life satisfaction scores averaged 24.25 (range = 5 to 35). Happiness scores were not significantly different among those with (mean = 4.89, SD = 1.27) or without (mean = 4.89, SD = 1.31) a university degree, but those with a degree had significantly higher life satisfaction scores (mean = 24.78, SD = 6.65) compared to those without a degree (mean = 23.09, SD = 7.36), t(2591) = 6.99, p  < 0.001. Further information to wellbeing, educational attainment, and intelligence can be found in the supplementary (see Supplementary Tables 9 and 10 ).

Testing linear associations

Analyses revealed that higher educational attainment, indexed by having at least a university degree, was not associated with subjective happiness, but did predict increased life satisfaction (Table 2 ). After including adjustments for main and interactive effects of sex, findings showed that females who completed university had significantly higher life satisfaction than those who did not, with differences appearing greater than those noted between males with and without a degree (see Fig. 3 ). For subjective happiness, the direction of effects was the opposite for the two sexes, with females more likely to experience positive benefits to their subjective happiness if they completed university, whereas male graduates were more at risk of lower subjective happiness (see Fig. 3 ). These opposing results likely explain the absence of effects noted in models unadjusted for sex.

figure 3

This figure shows differences between male and female subjective happiness and life satisfaction for those with higher intelligence (1 SD above the mean), and differences in subjective happiness for those with and without a university degree, and for life satisfaction for those with a degree.

Unadjusted models exploring the impact of intelligence revealed that as intelligence scores increased, subjective happiness declined, while life satisfaction increased (Table 2 ). After adding an interaction term between intelligence and sex, associations with life satisfaction remained, and associations with subjective happiness became positive. This suggests a moderating effect of sex, which is supported by plots of the findings (see Fig. 3 ). Males scoring more highly on the subjective happiness scale were those with lower intelligence scores (see Fig. 3 ).

All findings, including those for intelligence and educational attainment, remained after adjustment for multiple testing, and all findings replicated after adjustment for attrition (see Table 2 for adjusted results, and Supplementary Table 11 for unstandardised estimates).

Testing non-linear associations and moderating effects

When testing the relationship between intelligence and wellbeing for non-linearity, there was no evidence to suggest that non-linear models fit the data better than the linear models (see Supplementary Table 12 ). There was also no clear evidence to suggest moderating effects of educational attainment, with no strong interactions found between educational attainment and intelligence in analyses predicting subjective happiness (β = 0.001, SE = 0.003, p  = 0.721) or life satisfaction (β = 0.024, SE = 0.018, p  = 0.197). Analyses also revealed that family income is unlikely to explain associations between educational attainment and wellbeing, and between intelligence and wellbeing (see Supplementary Table 13 ).

This study was the first to combine genetic and observational data to test for causal associations between educational attainment, intelligence, and wellbeing. The MR results suggest that the relationship between educational attainment and wellbeing is bidirectional, with the magnitude of effects greater for wellbeing on educational attainment than vice versa. Findings also revealed that the causal and protective effect of staying in school is independent of intelligence, but may be greater for females relative to males.

Investigations into intelligence showed that wellbeing has a positive causal impact on intelligence, but intelligence a negative impact on wellbeing. These negative effects were only found after adjusting for educational attainment, implying either a direct and independent role, or that independent effects are in the opposite direction to the combined effects. Observational findings confirmed the direction of this effect for associations with subjective happiness but not life satisfaction, however, as per educational attainment, there were underlying sex differences. Together the findings stress the importance of staying in education over and above cognitive abilities for wellbeing.

Our MR finding that individuals who are genetically inclined to stay on and complete more years of schooling will have greater wellbeing was implied in previous observational studies 1 , 19 but not in a previous MR study 9 . The previous MR study found little impact of educational attainment on happiness. This was not found using positive affect in our MR analyses, however, previous findings do align with the current observational findings. These suggested that completing more years of schooling may positively impact life satisfaction but not happiness, however, males and females may be affected differently. The previous MR study by Davies and colleagues 9 adjusted for sex differences, which likely explains the different results to our MR findings.

Similar sex differences to our study have been reported previously in observational studies, with associations between schooling and happiness stronger among females relative to males 8 . This study in combination with the present findings suggest that females gain more to their wellbeing from continuing their education compared to males. One explanation for this could be due to underlying differences in socialisation.

Studies have shown that socialising has a greater impact on happiness among females relative to males 25 . Education has been referred to as an “institutionalised form of social resource” 4 and is an important determinant of social relations 26 . Spending more years in education therefore brings increased opportunities for not only developing cognitive skills, but also wider cultural awareness and social networks 4 . It is possible that this increased socialisation explains why females respond more positively to prolonged education than males.

Another possibility is that spending more years in education alters habits, practices, and health-related choices more favourably among females. Individuals genetically inclined to complete more years of schooling are more likely to engage in vigorous physical activity and less likely to engage in sedentary behaviour 3 . Educated females but not males have also been shown to be at a reduced risk of obesity 27 . Given the positive associations between BMI and wellbeing 15 , it is possible that sex differences in health behaviours contribute to the differential gains in the impact of education on wellbeing. Further research should attempt to understand these sex differences further to ensure more targeted support for males and females in schools. It is possible that males who remain in higher education would benefit from additional wellbeing support compared to females.

Our findings for intelligence add to the literature by providing causal evidence of the previously demonstrated negative associations with wellbeing 12 , 13 . In line with the current study, previous research also revealed a switch from positive to negative associations between intelligence and wellbeing after controlling for later-life outcomes like education attainment 12 . It has previously been suggested that this “residual” effect of intelligence on wellbeing may reflect the greater expectations of those high in intelligence with more education 12 . However, unlike previous observational research, the current findings were able to more directly rule out confounding of educational attainment to establish a causal and independent role for intelligence. The findings suggest it is possible that while educational attainment serves a protective function for those high in intelligence, the negative impact of lack of education is most detrimental for those with high intelligence. In other words, intelligence may negatively impact wellbeing among those who do not stay in education and who may be viewed as under-achievers.

The direct negative impact of intelligence on wellbeing may also reflect an underlying predisposition towards rumination and worry that is often reported among highly intelligent individuals 28 . It has been suggested that those high in intelligence have exaggerated physiological, neurological, and psychological responses to environmental stress that puts them at increased risk of mental health problems 29 . These reactions are more prevalent among those at the extreme end of the intelligence scale, which may explain why analyses using intelligence, but not educational attainment, produced negative associations with wellbeing. It is likely that such pupils may feel increased academic strain and pressure, and would benefit from additional wellbeing support at school.

It is also possible that different health behaviours underlie those high in intelligence compared to those who chose to stay on in higher education. Genetic studies of intelligence have revealed that unlike educational attainment, a genetic disposition towards higher intelligence is associated with reduced vigorous physical activity 3 . Intense physical activity is positively related to wellbeing across the lifespan 29 , 30 and may therefore explain the positive association between education and wellbeing, and negative association between education and intelligence.

The finding that higher wellbeing positively predicts both intelligence and years of schooling aligns with previous research which has shown that adolescents with increased wellbeing tend to perform better academically 31 . By using a causal design, the current study reduces bias from reverse causality and confounding to provide support for improving wellbeing in schools 32 . The finding that wellbeing and educational attainment have a bidirectional relationship suggests that interventions aimed at improving wellbeing in schools could encourage further education and improved cognitive skills, and these in turn, could improve wellbeing in later life. Similarly, by keeping students engaged in school and increasing the likelihood of further education, wellbeing is likely to be improved, which could further increase the potential for higher education. Together these findings highlight their reciprocal relationship.

This study used both genetic and observational data to triangulate and provide further insight into associations between educational attainment, intelligence, and wellbeing. By using both univariable and multivariable MR, our study was able to investigate whether causal relations reflect direct or indirect effects. This is particularly important as the longer an individual spends in schooling, the greater their adult intelligence 33 . Thus, by using a multivariable design it was possible to separate such effects. Observational analyses were also adjusted for attrition and selective participation, helping to reduce the potential for bias. Some limitations of this study, however should be noted.

The first is that the MR analyses used GWAS data that included large samples from the UK Biobank 16 . Participants in the UK Biobank are generally more educated than the general population, which may have reduced the generalisability of the causal effect estimates. Given the cost of education in several of the studies contributing to the GWASs (that identify the genetic instrument), the effects of education may also be picking up socioeconomic effects. Previous MR studies on educational attainment have shown that after reweighting for sample selection, there is minimal impact of educational biases on the overall estimates 9 . Nevertheless, it is important that findings are interpreted in light of this potential selection bias, and that researchers are mindful of possible confounding by socioeconomic status.

The MR findings should also be interpreted in light of assortative mating and dynastic effects. Findings have shown that individuals are more likely to select a mate with a similar educational background 34 and intelligence level 35 . This can lead to enriched educational or intelligence associated SNPs, as previously shown 36 , and may inflate subsequent MR estimates 37 . Dynastic effects can also bias MR estimates. Research has shown that parental educational level and family socioeconomic status predict the educational outcomes of their offspring 38 . Such dynastic effects as well as assortative mating can be investigated using a within-family design that adjusts for transmitted and non-transmitted alleles 39 . However, this was not possible in the current study as sufficient genotyped family data were not available. Nevertheless, a consistent result across MR estimates and observational analyses reduces the likelihood that MR estimates are confounding by characteristics that are transmitted across generations.

Further limitations of the current MR findings are that effect sizes relating to wellbeing are difficult to interpret. This is due to the nature of the meta-analytic findings which use multiple measures and phenotypes. While this is useful in testing whether or not there are possible causal effects, additional analyses using other methods are needed to estimate effect sizes. In addition, MR results using the intelligence GWAS from ref. 40 . used UKBiobank samples that conditioned on socioeconomic status. Sensitivity analyses conducted after removing these samples produced consistent results but it is important that main analyses are interpreted with some caution.

The current observational findings should also be interpreted in light of some limitations. The only available information relating to educational attainment was whether or not individuals had at least a university degree. While detailed information has recently been collected on educational qualifications in ALSPAC for this age group, this data has not yet been released. Analyses were therefore unable to explore non-linear or cumulative effects of years of schooling, meaning it is not possible to ascertain whether a particular level of education confers an advantage or disadvantage for wellbeing. Such knowledge could have important implications for guiding and supporting students who continue their education to post-graduate level. Nevertheless, previous findings have shown that using years of education or a “Graduates versus non-graduates” proxy of education makes minimal difference to overall results 2 .

Other possible limitations are that wellbeing was assessed at 26 years, four years after the average person graduates from university. While research has shown that the gap in happiness between the educated and less educated widens as individuals age, this gap does not appear until around 35 years of age 41 . This is suggested to reflect a time in which uncertainties and student loan debt repayments may be reduced. Further longitudinal research should explore trajectories of mental health and wellbeing following completion of higher education to gain a more in-depth understanding of the long-term outcomes of education. This could also aid insight into differences noted between associations with educational attainment and either subjective happiness or life satisfaction.

Unlike subjective happiness, life satisfaction captures cognitive evaluations of one’s life. When reporting on life satisfaction, participants are therefore required to draw comparisons between their actual and desired life situation. It is possible that positive effects of educational attainment and intelligence on life satisfaction therefore reflect the fulfilment of years of hard work. Indeed, findings have shown that factors related to individual prosperity, including income and possessions, predict increased life satisfaction but not feelings of happiness 42 . Measures of subjective happiness do not require cognitive processing but capture immediate and accessible feelings of pleasure. Such feelings may be less influenced by the accumulation of factors gained from education and more influenced by immediate sensations like perceived general health. Young adults in the current study may have been transitioning into their new role in either employment, parenthood, or another life domain, and thus have been exposed to increased stress. This could have resulted in lower happiness levels at that time. Further investigation into the role of educational attainment on subjective happiness at earlier or later stages of life may lead to different estimates.

Overall, the findings from this study suggest important avenues for further research. While steps were taken to triangulate and improve the interpretation of the MR results, future research should consider using repeated measures of wellbeing to understand how causal effects may unfold over time. Research should also attempt to understand the factors underlying positive effects of educational attainment on wellbeing, and should consider additional mediating factors. This will be key to further dissecting the causal pathway and could reveal subtle differences between predictors of life satisfaction and subjective happiness 42 , and factors specific to wellbeing at specific life stages.

The degree to which educational attainment is driven by educational achievement (the grades you get) or other non-cognitive skills also requires further investigation. Unlike educational attainment, educational achievement is assessed using test and examination results. While highly correlated with cognitive ability 43 , educational outcomes reflect more than just intelligence 44 , 45 . These non-cognitive abilities, such as self-control, emotion regulation, grit and motivation, may explain why some remain in education where others do not, even if they do not excel academically or intellectually. Understanding more about the educational attainment phenotype and its drivers could yield important insight into why effects of educational attainment and intelligence may differ. This could have implications for both intervention and policy.

A final priority for further study is to ensure replication in other countries and among other ancestries. The average number of years spent in education differs worldwide 46 , and there exists significant global variability in wellbeing across sex 47 . Wellbeing has also changed over time, with some evidence to suggest population declines in subjective happiness 48 . The current observational findings are limited to individuals born between 1991 and 1992. Those following the typical education trajectory would therefore have graduated from university in 2012 or 2013. Research has shown that the time in which an individual graduates can predict wellbeing, with those graduating in times of higher unemployment more likely to have lower life satisfaction 49 . This needs to be accounted for when investigating more recent effects of educational attainment, particularly in light of the COVID-19 pandemic and on-going economic uncertainty. The pandemic caused significant distress to many due to unprecedented changes to economic situations and education systems. The implications of which for young people’s future education is not certain but remains a public health priority. Our findings add further weight to this and stress the importance of staying in school over and above cognitive abilities for good wellbeing.

To conclude, our findings demonstrate a unique benefit for wellbeing of staying in school, over and above improving cognitive abilities. Benefits are likely to be greater for females relative to males, suggesting other interventions may be necessary to improve the wellbeing of males who remain in education. The finding that intelligence has a direct negative impact on wellbeing suggests that students high in intelligence may be at risk of increased academic stress, and may therefore benefit from additional wellbeing support to alleviate these pressures. Schools aiming to improve student wellbeing more widely should focus less on improving cognitive abilities, and more on keeping students engaged in school.

Principles of Mendelian Randomisation

Mendelian Randomisation (MR) is an instrumental variable method that uses natural genetic variation to study the causal effect of an exposure on an outcome 50 , 51 . The principles rely on Mendel’s law of segregation and independent assortment such that individuals inherit alleles that are independent of confounding traits and are randomly allocated at conception. Much like a Randomised Control Trial (RCT), the random segregation of participants, or alleles in the case of MR, are independent of any confounding variables, meaning confounding factors are assumed to be balanced across the two groups (see Fig. 4 taken from Davey Smith and Ebrahim 52 ). Any differences that arise are therefore attributed to causal effects, providing that certain assumptions are met.

figure 4

Analogy between Mendelian randomisation (MR) and randomsed controlled trial (RCT).

Assumptions of MR

MR is based on the three key assumptions; (1) The instrument must be robustly associated with the exposure of interest; (2) The instrument must not be associated with factors that may confound the association between the exposure and the outcome; (3) If there is a causal effect of an exposure on an outcome, then genetic variants associated with the exposure should also predict the outcome, through the exposure only. If this last assumption is violated and genetic variants act on a second exposure that influences the outcome, this is known as pleiotropy. Some forms of pleiotropy, such as vertical pleiotropy, satisfy the principles of MR and do not inflict bias. This is because such pleiotropy occurs when genetic variants predict a primary and a secondary exposure which are both on the same causal pathway to the outcome (see Fig. 5a ). This is the mechanism assumed in MR. If, however, the genetic variants act on the second exposure through a pathway other than through the primary exposure, this is known as horizontal pleiotropy (see Fig. 4b ). This can lead to biased estimates in MR if not accounted for.

figure 5

A Directed Acyclic Graph (DAG) demonstrating vertical and horizontal pleiotropy in associations between an exposure and an outcome.

Many of the above assumptions rarely hold in MR, particularly where large numbers of genetic variants, whose functions are often unknown, are used as instruments 53 . This is because these can make pleiotropic pathways more likely. Fortunately, there are measures that can be taken to improve the reliability of MR, including running alternative versions of MR that make different assumptions about pleiotropy, as well as multivariable MR 54 .

In traditional univariable MR, where the total effects of an exposure are investigated on an outcome, a second highly correlated exposure that is influenced by the same genetic variants would violate the assumptions of MR. An extension of MR, known as multivariable MR, allows exposures to be causally related provided the effects of the genetic variants are independent of the outcome 21 . Such an approach allows investigation into whether the two correlated exposures are causally related to the outcome, and whether such associations are independent of one another.

Genetic data

To conduct an MR study, researchers must decide whether to use a one- or two-sample approach. Two sample MR requires two independent study samples, one is used to provide estimates for associations between genetic markers and the exposure, and the other for associations between genetic markers and the outcome. Benefit of using a two-sample approach include that it provides more power and pleiotropy sensitivity analyses. However, it comes with the additional assumption that the two samples represent separate participants from similar populations 3 . Genome wide association studies for this MR study were therefore carefully selected to ensure sample overlap was minimal.

Data for educational attainment was taken from the Years of Schooling GWAS 17 This meta-analysed summary statistics from 64 samples, covering 15 different countries, all of European descent. Years of schooling were mapped and categorised across samples according to the 1997 International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) scale 55 . This initial analysis identified 74 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) that were independently associated with years of schooling ( m  = 14.3, SD  = 3.6) after adjustment for sex and ancestry principal components. A polygenic score constructed from the measured SNPs explained around 3.2% of the variance in educational attainment.

GWAS data were subsequently combined with those of 111,349 participants from the UK-Biobank (UKB) 17 . This replication resulted in a GWAS sample of 405,072 participants, and increased the number of associated genetic variants from 74 to 162. The current study, however, used data from the original discovery GWAS as opposed to the larger replication to reduce sample overlap (from 34% to 9%). Analyses were repeated using the larger replication cohort to ensure consistency (see Supplementary Table 14 ). It is important to note that while a larger and more recent meta-GWAS is available for educational attainment 56 , these samples largely overlap with those of the wellbeing GWAS used in the present study. Sensitivity analyses using MRLap, which is a method that accounts for potential sample overlap 57 , suggested estimates for univariable MR were similar to those using SNP estimates including23andMe (see Supplementary Table 15 ).

For intelligence, data were derived from the largest GWAS of intelligence to date ( n  = 269,867) 40 . This study was based on 14 cohorts that assessed intelligence using various neurocognitive tests of logical, verbal, spatial, and technical ability. Despite the different assessments, all cohorts extracted a single sum, mean, or factor score which was used to index general intelligence. Correlations across cohort measures were on average 0.67. Overall, the GWAS identified 242 lead SNPs associated with intelligence at genome-wide significance. Polygenic scores derived from these SNPs explained up to 5.2% of the variance in intelligence in four independent samples 40 .

Wellbeing data were taken from a multivariate genome-wide-association meta-analysis (GWAMA) of wellbeing 16 . This used the widely documented genetic overlap between four traits, life satisfaction, positive affect, depression, and neuroticism, to identify genetic variants associated with wellbeing. Two novel and complementary methods: An N-weighted multivariate GWAMA (N-GWAMA) and a model-averaging GWAMA (MA-GWAMA) approach were used. The N-GWAMA investigated a unitary effect of all traits, referred to collectively as the wellbeing spectrum, while MA-GWAMA relaxed the assumption of a unitary effect to study trait-specific estimates. Findings from the N-GWAMA revealed 231 independent SNPs associated with the wellbeing spectrum, while the MA-GWAMA resulted in 148 independent loci for life satisfaction and 191 for positive affect. The incremental R 2 for these SNPs was slightly lower than those derived from the N-GWAMA, therefore the current study used estimates related to the wellbeing spectrum. In particular, polygenic scores constructed from the N-GWAMA and MA-GWAMA explained 0.94% and 0.92% of the variance in life satisfaction and 1.10% and 1.06 of the variance in positive affect. Follow-up analyses were carried out to explore specific estimates for the individual wellbeing components (Supplementary Tables 5 to 8 ).

Approximately 11% of the individuals from the wellbeing GWAS sample were also included in the educational attainment GWAS sample 17 , and around 8% in the intelligence GWAS sample 40 . This overlap is similar to previous MR studies investigating wellbeing 15 .

Genetic instrument construction

Genetic variants included were those that passed the genome-wide level of significance ( p  < 5 × 10 −8 ) and were independent. Clumping was performed to ensure independence at r 2 < 0.001 within an 10,000 kb window. Data harmonisation was then performed using the TwoSampleMR package 58 , where allele frequencies were used to align palindromic SNPs down to a minor allele frequency of 0.42. For univariable MR analyses, instrument strength was calculated using an F statistic greater than 10 59 . For multivariable analyses, the Sanderson–Windmeijer partial F-statistic was used 60 .

Following data harmonisation, analyses exploring causal effects of educational attainment on intelligence used a total of 63 SNPs ( F  = 38.44), while analyses exploring the impact of intelligence on educational attainment used 144 SNPs ( F  = 42.72). Note that an F greater than 10 indicates the analysis is unlikely to suffer from bias due to a weak instrument 61 .

Analyses exploring the total causal effects of educational attainment on wellbeing used 54 SNPs that were available following data harmonisation ( F  = 38.88). For analyses exploring total causal effects of wellbeing on educational attainment, there were 147 SNPs available following data harmonisation ( F  = 40.78). Of these, 90 SNPs (61.2%) formed part of the original 232 SNPs identified in the wellbeing GWAS.

Analyses testing causal effects of intelligence on wellbeing used 126 SNPs following data harmonisation ( F  = 43.35). Analyses testing causal effects of wellbeing on intelligence used 128 SNPs ( F  = 40.83), of which 71 (55.4%) formed part of the original 232 lead SNPs in wellbeing GWAS.

As per the univariable MR analyses, variants were selected for the multivariable MR if they passed the genome-wide level of significance ( p  < 0.001 and 10,000 kb were used as conditions of clumping), and palindromic SNPs were aligned using a minor allele frequency of 0.42. Note that SNPS selected for multivariable MR are the exposure SNPs that are associated with the outcome, conditional on the other exposure. This resulted in 151 SNPs available for the multivariable MR analysis, a full list of which can be found in the Supplementary Table 14 .

Statistical analyses

Univariable mr.

Univariable MR analyses were conducted using the TwoSampleMR package, version 0.5.6 in R 61 . These analyses were used to test for causal associations between educational attainment and intelligence, and between wellbeing and the two exposures: educational attainment and intelligence. All univariable analyses were run using four different versions of two-sample MR. The inverse variance weighted (IVW) method was used as the main analysis as this assumes no directional pleiotropy, with sensitivity analyses including mendelian randomisation-Egger (MR-Egger), weighted median, and weighted mode. These have each been described in detail elsewhere 62 , 63 , but were included here as each makes a different assumption about pleiotropy. A consistent effect across the different methods can therefore provide more confidence that the assumptions are valid. In addition, a simulation extrapolation (SIMEX) correction was applied to MR-Egger estimates to correct coefficients where regression dilution was lower than 0.9 54 . A consistent result across these provides further support for a true causal effect.

To further assess the robustness of the results, heterogeneity was estimated using Cochran’s Q 64 . Tests of heterogeneity reveal how consistent the causal estimate is across SNPs, which can be used as an indicator of pleiotropy. Based on previous findings, it was anticipated that heterogeneity would be high 11 . High pleiotropy will only impose bias if it is directional and horizontal, and therefore the MR Egger intercept was used to check for evidence of directional/horizontal pleiotropy. A multiplicative random effects IVW regression was also chosen to adjust for this. Steiger filtering was conducted where more than one SNP explained more of the variance in the outcome than the exposure, which could suggest possible reverse causation.

As a sensitivity check, we repeated analyses using the intelligence GWAS without the UKB. This was because samples using the UKB conditioned on socioeconomic status.

Multivariable MR was then used to estimate the direct effects of educational attainment and intelligence on wellbeing, independent of the other. We also performed two additional multivariable MR analyses: (1) intelligence and well-being on years of schooling; (2) years of schooling and well-being on intelligence. All analyses were run using the MVMR package 21 and the MendelianRandomization package (Rees et al., 2017) in R. As per the univariable analyses, heterogeneity was checked using Cochran’s Q, and conditional F statistics using the Sanderson–Windmeijer partial F-statistic 60 .

Observational analyses

Observational data were taken from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC 24 ) a prospective cohort study based in the United Kingdom. Pregnant women residing in the former Avon area were enrolled if they had an expected delivery date between April 1991 and December 1992 65 . The initial cohort consisted of 14,062 live births but has since increased to 14,901 children following further recruitment 66 . Data gathered from 22 years and onwards were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted at the University of Bristol 67 . REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture) is a secure, web-based software platform designed to support data capture for research studies. Please note that the study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool ( http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/ ).

Participants included in the current study were those who completed a measure of educational attainment at age 26, an intelligence assessment at age 8, as well as relevant wellbeing measures at age 26 (see Supplementary Tables 10 , 15 , and 16 for further information about the measures, and Supplementary Fig. 4 for a flowchart of data availability). In total, there were 2844 participants with complete data on intelligence, wellbeing, and educational attainment. The wellbeing of participants with complete data on either intelligence ( n  = 3179) or educational attainment ( n  = 3788) did not differ (see Supplementary Table 9 ), therefore analyses were conducted on the two predictors separately to maximise power.

Ethical approval for the ALSPAC study was obtained from ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Informed consent for the use of data collected via questionnaires and clinics was obtained from participants following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee at the time.

Educational attainment was based on university degree completion. Participants responded to the item, ‘Do you have a university degree?’ which was included in the Life@26 questionnaire sent to 9230 (66%) participants in ALSPAC. While detailed information was collected on educational qualifications in ALSPAC for this age group, this data has not yet been released.

In total, 4029 completed the questionnaire, reflecting a 43.7% response rate. Answers included ‘yes’ ( n  = 2452), ‘no’ ( n  = 1377) or ‘still at university’ ( n  = 200). Those who responded ‘still at university’ were excluded from analyses. This is because individuals at university at 26 years would not necessarily represent those who followed the typical educational trajectory. For example, individuals may have taken a break from education and returned, or may be re-taking courses. Including such individuals may therefore have skewed analyses or created noise between the observational findings and those from the MR, which were based on years of schooling. Thus those with the highest number of total years would reflect those who earned a PhD degree at university. This could not be guaranteed among the current cohort of individual’s still studying due to the unavailability of further information.

Intelligence was assessed at the Focus at 8 clinic using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III 68 ). The WISC comprises of ten subtests, including five verbal tests and five performance tests, as well as a forwards/backwards digit span test. The overall continuous score represents the total scaled scores across verbal and performance tests which were calculated using the WISC manual.

Wellbeing was captured at 26 years using the Subjective Happiness Scale 69 , the Satisfaction with Life Scale 70 , and the Meaning in Life Scale 71 . The current study focused on the Subjective Happiness Scale and the Satisfaction with Life Scale to ensure a close replication of the MR study. The Subjective Happiness Scale includes 4 items, with overall scores reflective of greater subjective happiness. The scale has high internal consistency and test-retest reliability, and is suitable for different age, occupational, and cultural groups 69 . The Satisfaction with Life Scale is a 5-item measure that was designed to capture cognitive judgments of one’s life satisfaction as opposed to positive affect 70 . Answers are coded so that a higher overall score reflects greater life satisfaction. Correlations between life satisfaction and subjective happiness were r  = 0.65. Both wellbeing measures were z-standardised to facilitate comparisons between the two.

In an attempt to first replicate the MR findings, separate linear regression models were first run. These investigated associations between educational attainment and wellbeing, and between intelligence and wellbeing. Wellbeing was assessed using subjective happiness and life satisfaction, with each ran as a separate regression. Analyses were repeated after including an interaction between sex and the predictor to test for possible sex differences.

All linear models were corrected for multiple testing using Benjamini Hochberg False Discovery Rate (FDR 22 ). This was based on a total of 62 tests to include models adjusted for attrition and missing data.

As educational attainment was recorded using a binary response, analyses checking for possible non-linearity were conducted for intelligence only. Models investigating associations between intelligence and wellbeing included either a quadratic, cubic, or quartic polynomial term, as per previous research focused on mental health in young adulthood 72 . Additional analyses also explored non-linearity using spline regressions. This is because polynomial terms may not be flexible enough to capture the relationship between intelligence and wellbeing as they impose a global structure on all of the data. Spline regressions were therefore included within a Generalised Additive Model (GAM) which was run using the ‘mgcv’ R package 73 . The model of best fit was determined using the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC), as previously recommended 74 .

To further investigate possible factors driving associations with wellbeing, a linear model was run with an interaction term between the two predictors (educational attainment*intelligence). This was used to provide insight into the extent to which the relationship between intelligence and wellbeing is moderated by educational attainment and vice versa. Two interaction models were run, one predicting subjective happiness and one predicting life satisfaction. Finally, to test if any associations were explained by income, as noted in previous studies 2 , we repeated analyses after adjustment for family income.

The impact of attrition in the observational analyses was investigated using inverse probability weighting (IPW) and multiple imputation, as per previous studies using ALSPAC 75 . Multiple imputation was conducted using the Chained Equations (MICE) package 76 . Based on Rubin’s rules 77 , 60 imputations were conducted. The variables selected to impute data have been previously associated with missingness in ALSPAC and can be found in Supplementary Table 16 . It was important that analyses accounted for missing data as there was some evidence to suggest selective attrition (see Supplementary Table 15 ).

Data availability

All data sources used for the MR SNP-exposure and SNP-outcome associations are publicly available. Summary data from the Okbay et al. 17 Years of Schooling GWAS were downloaded from the SSGAC website SSGAC Login (thessgac.com), and the summary data for the intelligence GWAS 40 were obtained from the CNCR website GWAS Summary Statistics | CTG (cncr.nl). Summary statistics for the wellbeing GWAS, excluding results from 23AndMe cohort, were downloaded from https://surfdrive.surf.nl/files/index.php/s/Ow1qCDpFT421ZOO The observational ALSPAC data used in this study is not publicly available because the informed consent does not allow data to be made freely available through any third party maintained public repository. Data used for this submission, however, can be made available on request to the ALSPAC Executive. Please refer to the ALSPAC data management plan which describes the policy regarding data sharing. This is through a system of managed open access. Full instructions for applying for data access can be found here: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/ . The ALSPAC study website contains details of all the data that are available ( http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/ ), and a comprehensive list of grants funding is also available on the ALSPAC website ( http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf ).

Oreopoulos, P. & Salvanes, K. G. Priceless: the nonpecuniary benefits of schooling. J. Econ. Perspect. 25 , 159–184 (2011).

Article   Google Scholar  

Powdthavee, N., Lekfuangfu, W. N. & Wooden, M. What’s the good of education on our overall quality of life? A simultaneous equation model of education and life satisfaction for Australia. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 54 , 10–21 (2015).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Davies, N. M. et al. Multivariable two-sample Mendelian randomization estimates of the effects of intelligence and education on health. eLife 8 , e43990 (2019).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Yuan, S., Xiong, Y., Michaëlsson, M., Michaëlsson, K. & Larsson, S. C. Genetically predicted education attainment in relation to somatic and mental health. Sci. Rep. 11 , 4296 (2021).

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

O’Donnell, G., Deaton, A., Durand, M., Halpern, D., & Layard, R. Wellbeing and policy (Legatum Institute: London, 2014).

Diener, E. Subjective well-being. The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am. Psychol. 55 , 34–43 (2000).

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Cuñado, J. & Pérez de Gracia, F. Does education affect happiness? Evidence for Spain. Soc. Indic. Res. 108 , 185–196 (2012).

Nikolaev, B. Does higher education increase hedonic and eudaimonic happiness? J. Happiness Stud. 19 , 483–504 (2018).

Google Scholar  

Davies, N. M., Dickson, M., Davey Smith, G., Windmeijer, F., & van den Berg, G. J. The causal effects of education on adult mortality, health, and income: evidence from Mendelian randomization and the raising of the school leaving age. Int. J. Epidemiol. dyad104 , https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyad104 (2023).

Colom, R., Karama, S., Jung, R. E. & Haier, R. J. Human intelligence and brain networks. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 12 , 489–501 (2010).

Anderson, E. L. et al. Education, intelligence and Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from a multivariable two-sample Mendelian randomization study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 0 , 1–10 (2020).

Clark, A. E. & Lee, T. Early-life correlates of later-life wellbeing: evidence from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 181 , 360–368 (2021).

Flèche, S., Lekfuangfu, W. N. & Clark, A. E. The long-lasting effects of family and childhood on adult wellbeing: evidence from British cohort data. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 181 , 290–311 (2021).

Davey Smith, G. & Hemani, G. Mendelian randomization: genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum. Mol. Genet. 23 , R89–R98 (2014).

Wootton, R. E. et al. Evaluation of the causal effects between subjective wellbeing and cardiometabolic health: mendelian randomisation study. BMJ 362 , k3788 (2018).

Baselmans, B. M. L. et al. Multivariate genome-wide analyses of the well-being spectrum. Nat. Genet. 51 , 445–451 (2019).

Okbay, A. et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat. Genet. 48 , 624–633 (2016).

Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. A. & Diener, E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol. Bull. 131 , 803855 (2005).

Salinas-Jiménez, M. M., Artés, J. & Salinas-Jiménez, J. How do educational attainment and occupational and wage-earner statuses affect life satisfaction? A gender perspective study. J. Happiness Stud. 14 , 367–388 (2013).

Major, J. T., Johnson, W. & Deary, I. J. Linear and nonlinear associations between general intelligence and personality in project TALENT. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 106 , 638–654 (2014).

Sanderson, E., Davey Smith, G., Windmeijer, F. & Bowden, F. An examination of multivariable Mendelian randomization in the single-sample and two-sample summary data settings. Int. J. Epidemiol. 48 , 713–727 (2019).

Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodol.) 57 , 289–300 (1995).

Adams, C. D. A multivariable Mendelian randomization to appraise the pleiotropy between intelligence, education, and bipolar disorder in relation to schizophrenia. Sci. Rep. 10 , 6018 (2020).

Boyd, A. et al. Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42 , 111–127 (2013).

Kroll, C. Different things make different people happy: examining social capital and subjective well-being by gender and parental status. Soc. Indic. Res. 104 , 157–177 (2011).

Witkow, M. R. & Fuligni, A. J. In-school versus out-of-school friendships and academic achievement among an ethnically diverse sample of adolescents. J. Res. Adolesc. 20 , 631–650 (2010).

Amin, V., Behrman, J. R., & Spector, T. D. Does More Schooling Improve Health Outcomes and Health Related Behaviors? Evidence from U.K. Twins. Econ. Educ. Rev. 35 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2013.04.004 (2013).

Karpinski, R. I., Kolb, A. M. K., Tetreault, N. A. & Borowski, T. B. High intelligence: a risk factor for psychological and physiological overexcitabilities. Intelligence 66 , 8–23 (2018).

Costigan, S. A., Lubans, D. R., Lonsdale, C., Sanders, T. & del Pozo Cruz, B. Associations between physical activity intensity and well-being in adolescents. Prevent. Med. 125 , 55–61 (2019).

Ku, P.-W., Fox, K. R., Liao, Y., Sun, W.-Y. & Chen, L.-J. Prospective associations of objectively assessed physical activity at different intensities with subjective well-being in older adults. Qual. Life Res. 25 , 2909–2919 (2016).

Kaya, M. & Erdem, C. Students’ well-being and academic achievement: a meta-analysis study. Child Indic. Res. 14 , 1743–1767 (2021).

Bonell, C. et al. Why schools should promote students’ health and wellbeing. BMJ 348 , g3078 (2014).

Ritchie, S. J. & Tucker-Drob, E. M. How much does education improve intelligence? A Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Sci. 29 , 1358–1369 (2018).

Domingue, B. W., Lie, H., Okbay, A. & Belsky, D. W. Genetic heterogeneity in depressive symptoms following the death of a spouse: polygenic score analysis of the US Health and Retirement Study. Am. J. Psychiatry 174 , 963–970 (2017).

Plomin, R. & Deary, I. Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol. Psychiatry 20 , 98–108 (2015).

Torvik, F. A. et al. Modeling assortative mating and genetic similarities between partners, siblings, and in-laws. Nat. Commun. 13 , 1108 (2022).

Hartwig, F. P., Davies, N. M. & Davey Smith, G. Bias in Mendelian randomization due to assortative mating. Genet. Epidemiol. 42 , 608–620 (2018).

Wang, B. et al. Genetic nurture effects on education: a systematic review and meta-analysis. bioRxiv . https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.01.15.426782 (2021).

Munafò, M. R., Davies, N. M. & Davey Smith, G. Can genetics reveal the causes and consequences of educational attainment? J. R. Stat. Soc., Ser. A 183 , 681–688 (2019).

Savage, J. E. et al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nat. Genet. 50 , 912–919 (2018).

Nikolaev, B. & Rusakov, P. Education and happiness: an alternative hypothesis. Appl. Econ. Lett. 23 , 827–830 (2015).

Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J. & Arora, R. Wealth and happiness across the world: material prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 99 , 52–61 (2010).

Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P. & Fernandes, C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence 35 , 13–21 (2007).

Krapohl, E. et al. The high heritability of educational achievement reflects many genetically influenced traits, not just intelligence. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111 , 15273–15278 (2014).

Demange, P. A. et al. Investigating the genetic architecture of noncognitive skills using GWAS-by-subtraction. Nat. Genet. 53 , 35–44 (2021).

Lee, J. J. et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome -wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 50 , 1112–1121 (2018).

Ruggeri, K., Garcia-Garzon, E., Maguire, Á., Matz, S. & Huppert, F. A. Well-being is more than happiness and life satisfaction: a multidimensional analysis of 21 countries. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 18 , 192 (2020).

Jebb, A. T., Morrison, M., Tay, L. & Diener, E. Subjective well-being around the world: trends and predictors across the Life Span. Psychol. Sci. 31 , 293–305 (2020).

Cutler, D. M., Huang, W. & Llera-Muney, A. When does education matter? The protective effect of education for cohorts graduating in bad times. Soc. Sci. Med. 127 , 63–73 (2015).

Davey Smith, G. & Hemani, G. Mendelian randomization: genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum. Mol. Genet . 23 , R89–R98 (2014).

Davies, N. M., Holmes, M. V. & Davey Smith, G. Reading Mendelian randomisation studies: a guide, glossary, and checklist for clinicians. BMJ 362 , k601 (2018).

Davey Smith, G., Ebrahim, S. Mendelian Randomization: Genetic Variants as Instruments for Strengthening Causal Inference in Observational Studies. In: Weinstein M., Vaupel J. W., Wachter K. W., (Eds). National Research Council (US) Committee on Advances in Collecting and Utilizing Biological Indicators and Genetic Information in Social Science Surveys; Biosocial Surveys, (336-366). Washington (DC): National Academies Press. (2008).

Bowden, J. et al. Assessing the suitability of summary data for two-sample Mendelian randomization analyses using MR-Egger regression: the role of the I2 statistic. Int. J. Epidemiol. 45 , 1961–1974 (2016).

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Hemani, G., Bowden, J. & Davey Smith, G. Evaluating the potential role of pleiotropy in Mendelian randomization studies. Hum. Mol. Genet. 27 , R195–R208 (2018).

UNESCO. International standard classification of education—ISCED 1997. In Advances in Cross-National Comparison (eds Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, J.H.P. & Wolf, C.) (2003, Springer, Boston, MA). http://www.uis.unesco.org/Library/Documents/isced97-en.pdf

Okbay, A. et al. Polygenic prediction of educational attainment within and between families from genome-wide association analyses in 3 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 54 , 437–449 (2022).

Mounier, N. & Kutalik, Z. Bias correction for inverse variance weighting Mendelian randomization. Genet. Epidemiol. 47 , 314–331 (2023).

Hemani, G. et al. The MR-Base platform supports systematic causal inference across the human phenome. eLife 7 , e34408 (2018).

Staiger, D. & Stock, J. H. Instrumental variables regression with weak instruments. Econometrica 65 , 557–586 (1997).

Sanderson, E. & Windmeijer, F. A weak instrument F-test in linear IV models with multiple endogenous variables. J. Econ. 190 , 212–221 (2016).

R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/ (2021).

Bowden, J., Davey Smith, G. & Burgess, S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int. J. Epidemiol. 44 , 512–525 (2015).

Burgess, S. et al. Guidelines for performing Mendelian randomization investigations. Wellcome Open Res. 4 , 186 (2019).

Bowden, J. et al. Improving the visualization, interpretation and analysis of two-sample summary data Mendelian randomization via the Radial plot and Radial regression. Int. J. Epidemiol. 47 , 1264–1278 (2018).

Fraser, A. et al. Cohort profile: the avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42 , 97–110 (2013).

Northstone, K. et al. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC): an update on the enrolled sample of index children in 2019. Wellcome Open Res. 4 , 51 (2019).

Harris, P. A. et al. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J. Biomed. Inform. 42 , 377–381 (2009).

Wechsler, D., Golombok, J., & Rust, J. WISC-III UK Wechsler intelligence scale for children: UK manual. The Psychological Corporation; Sidcup, UK. (1992).

Lyubomirsky, S. & Lepper, H. A measure of subjective happiness: preliminary reliability and construct validation. Soc. Indic. Res. 46 , 137–155 (1999).

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J. & Griffin, S. The satisfaction with life scale. J. Personal. Assess. 49 , 71–75 (1985).

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S. & Kaler, M. The meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J. Counseling Psychol. 53 , 80–93 (2006).

Kwong, A. et al. Identifying critical points of trajectories of depressive symptoms from childhood to young adulthood. J. Youth Adolesc. 48 , 815–827 (2019).

Wood, S. Generalized additive models: an introduction with R CRC Press: Florida, 2006).

Singer J. D., & Willett, J. B. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modelling Change and Event Occurence (Oxford University Press: New York, 2003).

Cornish, R. P., Tilling, K., Boyd, A., Davies, A. & Macleod, J. Using linked educational attainment data to reduce bias due to missing outcome data in estimates of the association between the duration of breastfeeding and IQ at 15 years. Int. J. Epidemiol. 44 , 937–945 (2015).

Van Buuren, S. & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J. Stat. Software 45 , 1–68 (2010).

Little, R. J., & Rubin, D. B. Statistical Analysis with Missing Data . (John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, 2014).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. Thank you also to the cohorts that made their GWAS summary data publicly available. The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website ( http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf ). J.M.A. is supported by the Wolfson Centre for Young People’s Mental Health at Cardiff University. R.E.W. is supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (2020024). C.M.A.H. is supported by a Philip Leverhulme Prize. This publication is the work of the authors, and JMA will serve as guarantor for the contents of this paper.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Wolfson Centre for Young People’s Mental Health, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales, UK

J. M. Armitage

School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK

R. E. Wootton & C. M. A. Haworth

Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway

R. E. Wootton

Bristol Medical School (PHS), University of Bristol, Bristol, UK

O. S. P. Davis

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

J.M.A. and C.M.A.H. defined the research question. Funding acquisition and data collection for the wellbeing material in ALSPAC were performed by C.M.A.H. Data curation, formal analysis and investigation were performed by J.M.A., R.E.W. assisted with the MR data analysis. J.M.A., R.E.W., C.M.A.H., and O.S.P.D. contributed to the interpretation of the data. The original draft of the manuscript was written by J.M.A., and all authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. M. Armitage .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information, rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Armitage, J.M., Wootton, R.E., Davis, O.S.P. et al. An exploration into the causal relationships between educational attainment, intelligence, and wellbeing: an observational and two-sample Mendelian randomisation study. npj Mental Health Res 3 , 23 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44184-024-00066-x

Download citation

Received : 06 March 2023

Accepted : 01 April 2024

Published : 09 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s44184-024-00066-x

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

research articles in educational psychology

research articles in educational psychology

Educational Psychology Review

Special Issues can be accessed through the Collections link on the right-hand side Educational Psychology Review is an international forum for the publication of peer-reviewed integrative review articles, special thematic issues, reflections or comments on previous research or new research directions, interviews, and research-based advice for practitioners - all pertaining to the field of educational psychology. The contents provide breadth of coverage appropriate to a wide readership in educational psychology and sufficient depth to inform the most learned specialists in the discipline.

This is a transformative journal , you may have access to funding.

Latest articles

Cracking chicken-egg conundrums: juxtaposing contemporaneous and lagged reciprocal effects models of academic self-concept and achievement’s directional ordering.

  • Herbert W. Marsh
  • Fernando Núñez-Regueiro

research articles in educational psychology

Investigating Success in the Transition to University: A Systematic Review of Personal Risk and Protective Factors Influencing Academic Achievement

  • Isabelle Ball
  • Moitree Banerjee
  • Ian Tyndall

research articles in educational psychology

The Development, Testing, and Refinement of Eccles, Wigfield, and Colleagues’ Situated Expectancy-Value Model of Achievement Performance and Choice

  • Jacquelynne S. Eccles
  • Allan Wigfield

research articles in educational psychology

The Interplay of Cognitive Load, Learners’ Resources and Self-regulation

  • Tina Seufert
  • Verena Hamm
  • Valentin Riemer

research articles in educational psychology

Revisiting Picture Functions in Multimedia Testing: A Systematic Narrative Review and Taxonomy Extension

  • Lauritz Schewior
  • Marlit Annalena Lindner

research articles in educational psychology

Journal information

  • Current Contents/Social & Behavioral Sciences
  • Google Scholar
  • Norwegian Register for Scientific Journals and Series
  • OCLC WorldCat Discovery Service
  • Social Science Citation Index
  • TD Net Discovery Service
  • UGC-CARE List (India)

Rights and permissions

Editorial policies

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Open Access. Powered by Scholars. Published by Universities. ®

Digital Commons Network

  • Digital Commons Network ™
  • / Education

Educational Psychology Commons ™

research articles in educational psychology

Popular Articles

Based on downloads in April 2024

Effects Of Student Athletics On Academic Performance , Nicole Grimit South Dakota State University

Effects Of Student Athletics On Academic Performance , Nicole Grimit

The journal of undergraduate research.

The purpose of this study is to examine the effects of involvement in athletics at the collegiate level at South Dakota State University. This paper seeks to determine if participation in collegiate athletics is beneficial to a person or if its disadvantages outweigh the advantages. Sixty-seven student-athletes completed a survey during October of 2013 to determine the effects that athletic involvement has had on SDSU’s campus. This study found that participation in athletics is extremely beneficial. These athletes performed better in the classroom, developed impressive time management skills, felt motivated to complete their degree, were motivated to attend classes, and …

Go to article

The Effect Of Rewards And Motivation On Student Achievement , Lori Kay Baranek Grand Valley State University

The Effect Of Rewards And Motivation On Student Achievement , Lori Kay Baranek

Masters theses.

A review of research is conducted to examine the effect of rewards on intrinsic motivation and learning for children in general education and special education. Intrinsically motivated students experience school success because they display behaviors such as choosing challenging activities and spending more time on task. The use of rewards undermines intrinsic motivation and results in the slower acquisition of skills and more errors in the learning process. The implications of the research are discussed and a collection of strategies designed to promote intrinsic motivation is presented. One part of the collection presents strategies for all students, and the other …

Exploring How Student Athletes Balance Athletic, Academic, And Personal Needs Through Learned Needs Theory. , Michael E. Rutledge II Louisiana State University

Exploring How Student Athletes Balance Athletic, Academic, And Personal Needs Through Learned Needs Theory. , Michael E. Rutledge Ii

Journal of research initiatives.

The attempt to balance the requirements of athletic and academic demands prompts extensive research agendas from higher education and athletic stakeholders to examine how extrinsic and socio-environmental factors affect the desired outcomes of student athletes. Reputable motivation literature describes needs as the starting point of motivation and influences behaviors embedded within cultural and systematic structures. Thus, the purpose of this study is to understand how sport participation influences athletic and academic performance through Learned Needs Theory (LNT). This study provides insight to processes of motivation that contribute to knowledge, practical implications, and research that translates to research-based approaches to increase …

Social Cognitive Theory , iSALT Team Minnesota State University - Mankato

Social Cognitive Theory , Isalt Team

Isalt resources: theories, concepts, and measures.

No abstract provided.

Educational Learning Theories , Molly Zhou, David Brown Dalton State College

Educational Learning Theories: 2nd Edition , Molly Zhou, David Brown

Education open textbooks.

This open textbook was the result of a remix of pre-existing open materials collected and reviewed by Molly Zhou and David Brown. Learning theories covered include the theories of Piaget, Bandura, Vygotsky, Kohlberg, Dewey, Bronfenbrenner, Eriksen, Gardner, Bloom, and Maslow. The textbook was revised in 2018 through a Round Ten Revisions and Ancillary Materials Mini-Grant .

  • Behaviorism
  • Cognitive Development
  • Social Cognitive Theory
  • Experiential Learning Theory
  • Human Motivation Theory
  • Information Processing Theory

The Effectiveness Of School Uniforms On Students' Academic Achievement And Overall Classroom Behavior , Bobbi Jo Adams Rowan University

The Effectiveness Of School Uniforms On Students' Academic Achievement And Overall Classroom Behavior , Bobbi Jo Adams

Theses and dissertations.

The purpose of the study was to see what effect school uniforms had on students' academics and behavior. Subjects were seventh and eighth grade students, from a school which recently implemented a uniform policy for the 2006-2007 school year. Their academic and discipline records were collected from the current and previous year. Students from the previous year, who were not required to wear uniforms, were placed in the control group (n=55). Students, who were now required to wear uniforms, were placed in the experimental group (n=55). A t -test for independent samples was run on the data. No significant difference …

The Effects Of Participation In Athletics On Academic Performance Among High School Sophomores And Juniors , Lee Sitkowski Liberty University

The Effects Of Participation In Athletics On Academic Performance Among High School Sophomores And Juniors , Lee Sitkowski

Doctoral dissertations and projects.

Athletics and academic performance has been studied at length over the years in the literature. Despite the mostly university level research conducted, no consensus has been reached regarding the impact of athletic participation on academic performance at the high school level. As a result, the relationship between the in season and out of season school academic performance of high school sophomores and juniors in one high school was investigated in this study. It was determined that there was a significant relationship that existed between academic performance, measured by GPA, and athletic participation. Through an analysis of 249 high school sophomore …

The Effects Of Mental Health In Students’ Academic And Social Success , Matthew Evans Northwestern College - Orange City

The Effects Of Mental Health In Students’ Academic And Social Success , Matthew Evans

Master's theses & capstone projects.

The percentage of children experience mental health issues such as anxiety, behavior disorders and depression have continued to become more prevalent in the last twenty years. The purpose of this literature review is to examine the prevalence and contributing factors of mental health disorders in school aged children and the effects it has on their social success and academic achievement and what can be done to support children to be successful and improve their mental health. Educators are a key factor in improving a child’s mental health, with much of a child’s time being spent in school, educators are more …

The Effects Of Technology On Student Motivation And Engagement In Classroom-Based Learning , James Francis University of New England

The Effects Of Technology On Student Motivation And Engagement In Classroom-Based Learning , James Francis

All theses and dissertations.

Technology has seen a recent widespread integration into daily life, where access to vast amounts of information is now available with ease. Today’s generation of students has grown up with technology all around them in an ever-increasing manner. To create an effective 21st century classroom that meets the needs of the students, a modern teacher must factor a student’s motivation to learn and the effects technology has on inclusionary education.

A technology implementation was devised to address this rising need. Research was completed at an urban charter school on a population of 348 at the time of technology intervention through …

A Correlational Study Of The Relationship Between Academic Performance And Parental Age , Jamie Melamed Rowan University

A Correlational Study Of The Relationship Between Academic Performance And Parental Age , Jamie Melamed

This study sought to examine the relationship between academic performance and parental age. Parental age has previously been shown to be a strong predictor of academic performance. This study consisted of a survey of 45 undergraduate students at Rowan University to determine the degree to which their parents' ages, both mothers' and fathers', correlated with academic performance as measured by high school class rank, SAT score, and GPA at Rowan. All participants were children of intact families, and the sample consisted of 25 females and 20 males. Both mother's age and father's age were found to correlate strongly with the …

All Articles in Educational Psychology

7,748 full-text articles. Page 1 of 284 .

“In My Blood”: External Factors For International Stem Postdoctoral Scholars’ Career Decisions , Kathryn J. Watson, Sylvia L. Mendez 2024 The University of Iowa

“In My Blood”: External Factors For International Stem Postdoctoral Scholars’ Career Decisions , Kathryn J. Watson, Sylvia L. Mendez

Journal of global education and research.

This instrumental case study (Stake, 1995) explores the external factors that influence international science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) postdoctoral scholars in the United States to pursue a career in STEM. Interviews with 20 international STEM postdoctoral scholars were analyzed deductively to shed light on their unique backgrounds and experiences. Three themes emerged: (a) parents were highly encouraging, (b) a love of science was nurtured in school, and (c) they were eager to engage in and promote scientific innovation. These findings illustrate the ways in which family, schools, and community influence the STEM career trajectories of international postdoctoral scholars.

Identifying Graduate Students’ Instructional Strategies And Approaches Towards Teaching Employable Skills , Elizabeth S. Che 2024 The Graduate Center, City University of New York

Identifying Graduate Students’ Instructional Strategies And Approaches Towards Teaching Employable Skills , Elizabeth S. Che

Dissertations, theses, and capstone projects.

College instruction tends to focus on imparting disciplinary knowledge rather than employable broad-based skills emphasized by undergraduate guidelines. The lack of emphasis on broad-skill development may leave many undergraduate students unprepared for the workforce. Graduate students who are future professoriate, are teaching undergraduate courses with various attitudes and strategies. This dissertation comprises three published studies that used data from two surveys identifying graduate students’ instructional strategies and approaches to teaching employable skills in their courses.

The first study asked whether graduate students teaching undergraduate courses ( N = 114; 70.2% women, M age = 30 years) aim to teach employable …

Adaptations And Transformations: Perceptions Of Change In Academic Motivation, Resilience, And Covid-19 Stress , Ella V. Gregorio 2024 City University of New York (CUNY)

Adaptations And Transformations: Perceptions Of Change In Academic Motivation, Resilience, And Covid-19 Stress , Ella V. Gregorio

Of concern to educators, post-COVID-19 assessments have revealed significant achievement gaps in student performance, as well as significant decreases in adolescent mental health. Less is known about changes in achievement motivation. Here we ask how students perceive their own motivation and whether it changed between “before” the pandemic to the present (Fall 2022 - Spring 2023), about three years after the beginning of the pandemic when many school routines were returning to “normal.” To understand how variance in these perceived changes might be related to pandemic-related stressors, trait resilience, and current mental health, we conducted a large survey study (N …

In My Softest & Most Liberatory Dreams: Reflections On Holding Complexity & Decentering Whiteness , Richard C. Clark 2024 The Graduate Center, City University of New York

In My Softest & Most Liberatory Dreams: Reflections On Holding Complexity & Decentering Whiteness , Richard C. Clark

As the world contends with a global pandemic, climate catastrophes, white supremacy, coloniality, and concurrent genocides my attention splinters. In an act of futurity, or future making, I ask myself: What is needed to move from this place toward softer, more liberatory futures? This body of work finds its answer in exploring two interrelated concepts: Decentering Whiteness and Holding Complexity . Decentering Whiteness is the process of working toward a future where all the personal, spiritual, educational, epistemological, social, structural, psychological, financial, and systemic ties to white supremacy are unraveled. Holding Complexity weaves together knowledges of care, accountability, intersectionality, and …

Me And Mathematics: “Doing What You’Re Talking About”: In Dialogue With My Family , Eden Morris 2024 The Graduate Center, City University of New York

Me And Mathematics: “Doing What You’Re Talking About”: In Dialogue With My Family , Eden Morris

This paper is a philosophically oriented accompaniment to my audio project (accessible through the following link: https://cuny.manifoldapp.org/projects/me-and-mathematics). Working together, the paper and audio collages form a call to action and a resource. My primary finding is the importance of doing what you’re talking about or exploring and implementing your ideas experientially. Doing what you’re talking about is important for effective teaching/learning and feeling in line with oneself. This working concept came to my attention during my research conversation with my oldest living relative, and then, again, with my youngest (non-baby) relative. This doing what you’re talking about is a way …

What Exactly Does Identity Have To Do With Teaching? Exploring The Connection Between A Teacher's Racialized Identity And Their Teacher Identity , Lizette Aguilar 2024 The Graduate Center, City University of New York

What Exactly Does Identity Have To Do With Teaching? Exploring The Connection Between A Teacher's Racialized Identity And Their Teacher Identity , Lizette Aguilar

This study explores the relationship between race and teacher identity, specifically understanding how the two concepts intersect and (re)present themselves within the classroom environment. This study analyzes six teachers’ narratives of identity, race, and teaching to explore themes of race and identity as they traverse from their earliest experiences with race into their experiences as teachers. Findings conclude that, first, teachers’ narrative identities are a composite of key narratives—important moments of deep impact in the process of identity building—that accumulate over time to form what Dan McAdams (1993) calls personal myths. Since key narratives are continually shaping one’s personal myth, …

Experiences Affecting Military Children's Performance In School: A Hermeneutic Phenomenology , Christopher Lee Pruitt Casely 2024 Liberty University

Experiences Affecting Military Children's Performance In School: A Hermeneutic Phenomenology , Christopher Lee Pruitt Casely

The purpose of this hermeneutic phenomenological study was to understand how attachment and loss associated with deployment affect children’s academic performance at Joint Base Lewis McChord. More specifically, the research was centered around a child's attachment to their parent and whether deployment affects that attachment or their academic success. The theory guiding this study was John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth's attachment theory, as it provided a unique framework to examine how the child is affected by the deployment of their parents. The foundational question that drove this study was: What are the experiences of military families, specifically their children’s academic …

From Doom To Bloom: The Effects Of Pre-Major Coaching On Undecided Student Persistence , Lisa Dianne Wycoff 2024 Liberty University

From Doom To Bloom: The Effects Of Pre-Major Coaching On Undecided Student Persistence , Lisa Dianne Wycoff

Student retention is a persistent dilemma in higher education because it is how student success is measured. Universities invest resources in retaining vulnerable populations and provide additional support measures because they are most at risk of dropping out. The premise of this study is that students who enter college without a major are considered vulnerable, as they are highly prone to drop out. Numerous studies have found that entering college with an “unknown/undecided” status negatively impacts retention, well-being, and motivation due to a lack of clearly defined educational goals. However, students and families often lack adequate information about the importance …

The Impact Of School-Based Mentoring On At-Risk Students Of An Urban School District , Brandon B. Richardson 2024 Liberty University

The Impact Of School-Based Mentoring On At-Risk Students Of An Urban School District , Brandon B. Richardson

It is necessary to consider and implement a strategy to close the academic performance gap, particularly for at-risk adolescents in urban school districts. Mentoring programs have been around for a long time and continue to be successful. This research study's goal was to investigate the influence at-risk adolescents in an urban school district get from a school-based mentorship program. The study had a total of 40 participants (10 mentors, 8 parents, 10 teachers, 10 students, and 2 administrators). The study employed a mixed methods research strategy. Using a quantitative method approach, the researcher investigated whether a mentorship program had significant …

Moral Injury In Active Service And Veteran Female Military Combatants , William Curtis Neal 2024 Liberty University

Moral Injury In Active Service And Veteran Female Military Combatants , William Curtis Neal

A topic of concern and discussion in the mental health community, and maybe a special concern from both governmental and civilian practitioners, is the subject of moral injury. Moral Injury has been widely researched over the last 10 years and is a known and discussed condition that is linked to military personnel and veterans who have experienced events while deployed or operating in adverse conditions such as combat. The following descriptive study focuses on one group from which they have been excluded or overlooked in past studies. This creates a gap in the published literature: female servicewomen and veterans. Both …

Leadership And Teacher Agency For Inclusive Classrooms: Insights About Integrating Students With Disabilities Into Inclusive Classrooms From Teachers And School Leaders In Three International Schools , Chad Wood 2024 University of San Diego

Leadership And Teacher Agency For Inclusive Classrooms: Insights About Integrating Students With Disabilities Into Inclusive Classrooms From Teachers And School Leaders In Three International Schools , Chad Wood

Dissertations.

As international schools continue to flourish, they must consider how to be inclusive and meet the needs not only of multicultural populations of students, but also the needs of students with diagnosed learning difficulties (i.e., students with special needs). Promoting teacher agency is a potentially important component in successfully implementing inclusive practices given that teachers are key actors in the implementation process. Currently, however, there is limited empirical evidence regarding the interconnectedness of teacher agency and inclusion of special needs populations in international schools.

This qualitative research explores the role of school leaders in promoting teacher agency to support the …

Black School Leader Truth: How Black School Leader Epistemology Influences Liberatory Mindset Development In Their Staff , Orpheus Williams 2024 University of San Diego

Black School Leader Truth: How Black School Leader Epistemology Influences Liberatory Mindset Development In Their Staff , Orpheus Williams

Black students in the United States have consistently been denied access to empowering, culturally affirming, and responsive learning experiences in the traditional public system. The epistemological and pedagogical beliefs embedded in a liberatory mindset can subvert this pattern of disempowerment (Shujaa, 1998). Black school leaders (BSLs) who understand the systemic and institutional pressures Black children may face and have to overcome, having undergone similarly racialized experiences in school, are uniquely placed to create liberatory spaces for Black students by recruiting and developing these liberatory mindsets in their staff. This instrumental case study used qualitative research methods of front-porch pedagogy (McTighe …

Movin’ On Up: The Lived Experience Of Women Of Color In Leadership And The Supports That Helped Them Make It To The Top , Rozina Kapadia 2024 UMass Global

Movin’ On Up: The Lived Experience Of Women Of Color In Leadership And The Supports That Helped Them Make It To The Top , Rozina Kapadia

Purpose: The number of women of color in leadership continues to be significantly lower than the number of men. The lack of opportunity combined with the lack of support results in less women of color in leadership. The purpose of this phenomenological study was to explore the lived experiences of women of color in their roles as leaders in corporate environments with respect to the support (personal supports, organizational supports, cultural supports, and stereotypical supports) received. Specifically, the research examined the stories of female leaders of color to understand how their experiences have supported or hindered their growth in leadership. …

How Sensory Exploration Using Expressive Arts Provides A Cohesive Experience For Children With Multiple Diagnoses , Chanelle Goguen 2024 Lesley University

How Sensory Exploration Using Expressive Arts Provides A Cohesive Experience For Children With Multiple Diagnoses , Chanelle Goguen

Expressive therapies capstone theses.

This thesis looks at the potential benefits and cohesive experience of sensory exploration for children with multiple diagnoses through a trauma informed lens. The researcher was inspired by Prendiville’s (2021) idea of how interactive sensory exploration and art making can help foster and develop reciprocal communication and socio-affective relationships influenced the researcher to design a method that would be used to collect data about potential similar findings. In the method, the interaction of the sensory-specific art mediums were reflected through the expressive therapies continuum (ETC) framework. The research of this thesis acknowledges and explores how trauma and adverse childhood experiences …

Burnout Symptoms Among Millennial Teachers In India: The Efficacy Of The Emotional Self-Care Online Intervention Program (Escoip) , Joby Thomas, Marc Eric S. Reyes 2024 University of Santo Tomas, Manila, Philippines

Burnout Symptoms Among Millennial Teachers In India: The Efficacy Of The Emotional Self-Care Online Intervention Program (Escoip) , Joby Thomas, Marc Eric S. Reyes

Makara human behavior studies in asia.

The phenomenon of teacher burnout has gained greater attention due to its grave impact on the educational system and the well-being of teachers. In this regard, millennial teachers’ experiences— currently comprising most of the teaching workforce—have gained attention in scholarly research. Thus, developing an efficacious online intervention program is crucial to alleviate and avoid teacher burnout among millennials. This study investigated the effectiveness of the Emotional Self-Care Online Intervention Program (ESCOIP) in treating burnout symptoms among Indian millennial teachers. The present study employed Conklin’s program development model, which involves three phases: (1) planning, (2) design and implementation, and (3) evaluation …

The Meta-Analysis Of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Reliability , Naila N. Al Kharusi, Hussain Al Kharousi, Mai Helmy 2024 Department of Psychology, College of Education, Sultan Qaboos University, Sultanate of Oman

The Meta-Analysis Of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Reliability , Naila N. Al Kharusi, Hussain Al Kharousi, Mai Helmy

An-najah university journal for research - b (humanities).

The study aimed at meta-analysis of the reliability coefficients of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test scores to find out the typical estimate value of the reliability coefficient of test scores, and to reveal the various factors that may contribute to the high or low value of the reliability coefficient. A total of (36) studies were subjected to meta-analysis of reliability, where the number of reliability coefficients calculated were (206) reliability coefficients out of (16) indicators of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The current study focused on (98) reliability coefficients calculated by the method of test-retest reliability for six indicators. The results …

A Phenomenological Study Of Psychological Experience In The Mental Health Crisis Of Korean American Christian Older Adolescents , Munmi Son 2024 Liberty University

A Phenomenological Study Of Psychological Experience In The Mental Health Crisis Of Korean American Christian Older Adolescents , Munmi Son

The purpose of this phenomenological study is to understand the experience of psychological distress witnessed in the mental health crisis of Korean American Christian adolescents. Korean adolescents are at increased risk of attempting suicide. Therefore, it is important to understand the phenomenon and psychological experience of mental health problems among Korean Christian adolescents. This study explores Korean-American cultural characteristics and the psychology of access to mental health services by these adolescents with mental health concerns. The design of this study is an interpretative qualitative study and includes open semi-structured interviews and observations for data collection. This study identifies the distinctiveness …

Teacher Voices: Stress And Coping Mechanisms Among The Teaching Profession , Jasmine Suttles 2024 Dominican University of California

Teacher Voices: Stress And Coping Mechanisms Among The Teaching Profession , Jasmine Suttles

Education | master's theses.

Stress is a natural response that occurs in our body when we feel overwhelmed, and it can manifest in both positive and negative instances. The teaching profession is not exempt from this phenomenon, as it is unique in that educators have multiple roles to play and have to manage and interact with many students simultaneously. Teachers have been described as "exasperated by heavy loads of emotional labor in the classroom" (Ferguson et al., 2022). This "heavy load" can place the teaching profession at risk of stress and burnout if self-care is not addressed or mental health resources are not offered. …

Enhancing School Culture Through Positive Behavioral Intervention Supports (Pbis) Training And Implementation: Insights From Teacher And Administrator Educators , Grazia Benedetti 2024 Dominican University of California

Enhancing School Culture Through Positive Behavioral Intervention Supports (Pbis) Training And Implementation: Insights From Teacher And Administrator Educators , Grazia Benedetti

Positive Behavioral Intervention and Supports (PBIS) is a comprehensive approach to fostering a positive school environment by supporting social behavior throughout the school community. The goal of PBIS is to enhance both educational and social achievements for all students. Research has indicated that successful implementation of school-wide practices requires initiation that includes administrative engagement, creation of a leadership team, and alignment with the school’s vision and mission (Cook & Odom, 2013). Administrator support and ongoing professional development were crucial for the success of positive behavior interventions. School-wide PBIS, described by Angus and Nelson (2019), was a proactive approach to teaching …

Psychological Foundations Of Education , Joanna Maulbeck 2024 CUNY Kingsborough Community College

Psychological Foundations Of Education , Joanna Maulbeck

Open educational resources.

This is the syllabus that I used in Spring 2024 for EDC 2500: Psychological Foundations of Education . It is an OER course.

Popular Institutions

research articles in educational psychology

Featured Publications

Popular authors.

research articles in educational psychology

  • Adult and Continuing Education
  • Art Education
  • Bilingual, Multilingual, and Multicultural Education
  • Community College Leadership
  • Curriculum and Instruction
  • Curriculum and Social Inquiry
  • Disability and Equity in Education
  • Early Childhood Education
  • Educational Administration and Supervision
  • Educational Assessment, Evaluation, and Research
  • Educational Leadership
  • Educational Methods
  • Educational Technology
  • Education Economics
  • Elementary Education
  • Family and Consumer Sciences
  • Gifted Education
  • Health and Physical Education
  • Higher Education
  • Humane Education
  • Indigenous Education
  • Instructional Media Design
  • International and Comparative Education
  • Language and Literacy Education
  • Liberal Studies
  • Online and Distance Education
  • Other Education
  • Outdoor Education
  • Prison Education and Reentry
  • Science and Mathematics Education
  • Secondary Education
  • Social and Philosophical Foundations of Education
  • Special Education and Teaching
  • Student Counseling and Personnel Services
  • Teacher Education and Professional Development
  • Vocational Education

College of Education

stacey-mcelroy-heltzel-2019-17.jpg

McElroy-Heltzel’s research among International Journal of Psychology’s top cited articles

Stacey McElroy-Heltzel, University of Iowa assistant professor of counseling psychology and researcher in the field of psychology, has garnered recognition for her work on the impact of COVID-19 on mental health. Her article titled "Resource loss and mental health during COVID-19: Psychosocial protective factors among US older adults and those with chronic disease," published in the International Journal of Psychology , has been listed among the top 10 most-cited papers from January 1, 2022 through December 31, 2023.

McElroy-Heltzel's research delves into the intricate relationship between resource loss and mental health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic, with a particular focus on vulnerable populations such as older adults and individuals with chronic diseases. In the wake of unprecedented challenges posed by the pandemic, understanding the psychosocial protective factors that mitigate adverse mental health effects is of paramount importance.

“During the pandemic, the prevailing narrative was that we were a nation experiencing multiple crises, including a mental health crisis. While that was true in many ways, I think it's important that that's not the only narrative out there,” McElroy-Heltzel says. “We have a tendency to over pathologize normal responses to stressful events, and that's not a good message to send. It’s normal and appropriate to experience distress when bad things happen. While some people will develop significant mental health symptoms, many people experience both stress and resilience by drawing upon social support or having an optimistic outlook. It’s possible to recognize that while things are difficult at the moment, there is hope that things will get better.” 

The study conducted by McElroy-Heltzel and her team sheds light on the resilience exhibited by certain segments of the population amidst adversity. By examining factors such as social support, coping strategies, and access to essential resources, the research elucidates some protective factors that allow individuals to maintain psychological well-being in the face of resource constraints and heightened stressors.

The recognition of McElroy-Heltzel's work as one of the top 10 most-cited papers underscores its significance and impact within the scientific community. Her findings not only contribute to the academic discourse on mental health resilience but also have practical implications for policymakers, health care professionals, and community stakeholders striving to support vulnerable populations during public health crises.

In an era characterized by unprecedented global challenges, McElroy-Heltzel's research serves as a positive outlook, offering insights into the resilience of human spirit and the potential for collective action to foster positive mental health outcomes amidst adversity. As the world continues to navigate the complexities of the COVID-19 pandemic and its aftermath, the lessons gleaned from McElroy-Heltzel's work remain invaluable in shaping effective interventions and strategies to promote mental well-being for all.

“Many people find it difficult to reach out for social support during stressful times, especially if we believe everyone around us is distressed. We may fear being a burden. Yet, research has indicated that social support is one of the most consistent and robust correlates of psychological health. Therefore, we want to acknowledge that while many people are struggling, through connection, we can build resilience,” McElroy-Heltzel says. “Our findings support this idea. In this article, we were able to show how things like social support, resilience, and optimism mitigated some of the impacts of the pandemic related to factors on peoples’ mental distress, even for vulnerable populations.”

For McElroy-Heltzel, this recognition serves as a testament to the importance of interdisciplinary collaboration and rigorous scientific inquiry in addressing pressing societal issues. Moving forward, she remains committed to advancing research that not only informs policy and practice but also empowers individuals and communities to thrive in the face of adversity.

Sunny, summertime image featuring the Old Capitol

COE faculty recognized for excellence in research

UI faculty award medallion

Neal, Hoffman receive Collegiate Teaching Awards

Kenneth Brown in front of bookcase.

Brown honored with the Michael J. Brody Award for Faculty Excellence in Service

Frank T. McAndrew Ph.D.

How to Get Started on Your First Psychology Experiment

Acquiring even a little expertise in advance makes science research easier..

Updated May 16, 2024 | Reviewed by Ray Parker

  • Why Education Is Important
  • Find a Child Therapist
  • Students often struggle at the beginning of research projects—knowing how to begin.
  • Research projects can sometimes be inspired by everyday life or personal concerns.
  • Becoming something of an "expert" on a topic in advance makes designing a study go more smoothly.

ARENA Creative/Shutterstock

One of the most rewarding and frustrating parts of my long career as a psychology professor at a small liberal arts college has been guiding students through the senior capstone research experience required near the end of their college years. Each psychology major must conduct an independent experiment in which they collect data to test a hypothesis, analyze the data, write a research paper, and present their results at a college poster session or at a professional conference.

The rewarding part of the process is clear: The students' pride at seeing their poster on display and maybe even getting their name on an article in a professional journal allows us professors to get a glimpse of students being happy and excited—for a change. I also derive great satisfaction from watching a student discover that he or she has an aptitude for research and perhaps start shifting their career plans accordingly.

The frustrating part comes at the beginning of the research process when students are attempting to find a topic to work on. There is a lot of floundering around as students get stuck by doing something that seems to make sense: They begin by trying to “think up a study.”

The problem is that even if the student's research interest is driven by some very personal topic that is deeply relevant to their own life, they simply do not yet know enough to know where to begin. They do not know what has already been done by others, nor do they know how researchers typically attack that topic.

Students also tend to think in terms of mission statements (I want to cure eating disorders) rather than in terms of research questions (Why are people of some ages or genders more susceptible to eating disorders than others?).

Needless to say, attempting to solve a serious, long-standing societal problem in a few weeks while conducting one’s first psychology experiment can be a showstopper.

Even a Little Bit of Expertise Can Go a Long Way

My usual approach to helping students get past this floundering stage is to tell them to try to avoid thinking up a study altogether. Instead, I tell them to conceive of their mission as becoming an “expert” on some topic that they find interesting. They begin by reading journal articles, writing summaries of these articles, and talking to me about them. As the student learns more about the topic, our conversations become more sophisticated and interesting. Researchable questions begin to emerge, and soon, the student is ready to start writing a literature review that will sharpen the focus of their research question.

In short, even a little bit of expertise on a subject makes it infinitely easier to craft an experiment on that topic because the research done by others provides a framework into which the student can fit his or her own work.

This was a lesson I learned early in my career when I was working on my own undergraduate capstone experience. Faced with the necessity of coming up with a research topic and lacking any urgent personal issues that I was trying to resolve, I fell back on what little psychological expertise I had already accumulated.

In a previous psychology course, I had written a literature review on why some information fails to move from short-term memory into long-term memory. The journal articles that I had read for this paper relied primarily on laboratory studies with mice, and the debate that was going on between researchers who had produced different results in their labs revolved around subtle differences in the way that mice were released into the experimental apparatus in the studies.

Because I already had done some homework on this, I had a ready-made research question available: What if the experimental task was set up so that the researcher had no influence on how the mouse entered the apparatus at all? I was able to design a simple animal memory experiment that fit very nicely into the psychological literature that was already out there, and this prevented a lot of angst.

Please note that my undergraduate research project was guided by the “expertise” that I had already acquired rather than by a burning desire to solve some sort of personal or social problem. I guarantee that I had not been walking around as an undergraduate student worrying about why mice forget things, but I was nonetheless able to complete a fun and interesting study.

research articles in educational psychology

My first experiment may not have changed the world, but it successfully launched my research career, and I fondly remember it as I work with my students 50 years later.

Frank T. McAndrew Ph.D.

Frank McAndrew, Ph.D., is the Cornelia H. Dudley Professor of Psychology at Knox College.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • International
  • New Zealand
  • South Africa
  • Switzerland
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

May 2024 magazine cover

At any moment, someone’s aggravating behavior or our own bad luck can set us off on an emotional spiral that threatens to derail our entire day. Here’s how we can face our triggers with less reactivity so that we can get on with our lives.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) The Content of Educational Psychology: an Analysis of Top Ranked

    research articles in educational psychology

  2. (PDF) Guest Editorial: Methods for research in professional educational

    research articles in educational psychology

  3. (PDF) Educational Psychology's First Century

    research articles in educational psychology

  4. (PDF) Academic Vocabulary in Psychology Research Articles: A Corpus

    research articles in educational psychology

  5. 9781848723306: Educational Psychology (Topics in Applied Psychology

    research articles in educational psychology

  6. (PDF) The importance of Educational Psychology 1985

    research articles in educational psychology

VIDEO

  1. Study BSc Educational Psychology (Animation)

  2. ‘Recent trends of research in Educational Psychology’

  3. Educational psychology [Field, Methods, branches and Characteristics]

  4. Learning Science Research and High School Psychology

  5. History of Educational Psychology

  6. Ideas, Written Word, and Research: How to Publish in a Scholarly Journal

COMMENTS

  1. Journal of Educational Psychology

    The main purpose of the Journal of Educational Psychology® is to publish original, primary psychological research pertaining to education across all ages and educational levels. A secondary purpose of the journal is the occasional publication of exceptionally important meta-analysis articles that are pertinent to educational psychology.

  2. Latest articles from Educational Psychology

    The effect of a stress and anxiety coping programme on objective structured clinical exam performance in medical students, a randomised clinical trial. Bektas Murat Yalcin, Mustafa Unal, Hasan Pirdal & Tevfik Fikret Karahan. Published online: 09 Jun 2015. 1359 Views.

  3. Spotlight Articles in Educational Psychology, School Psychology, and

    Spotlight Articles. The multicultural play therapy room: Intentional decisions on toys and materials. from International Journal of Play Therapy. February 6, 2024. Finding your people and your space on the internet: The web as a source of support for trans students of color. from Journal of Diversity in Higher Education.

  4. Articles

    The Evolution of Race-Focused and Race-Reimaged Approaches in Educational Psychology: Future Directions for the Field. Jessica T. DeCuir-Gunby. Paul A. Schutz. REVIEW ARTICLE Open access 28 February 2024 Article: 30. Part of 1 collection:

  5. Educational Psychology: Vol 44, No 2 (Current issue)

    Educational Psychology, Volume 44, Issue 2 (2024) See all volumes and issues. ... Research Articles. Article. Social-emotional need satisfaction and students' academic engagement and social-emotional skills. Rebecca J. Collie. Pages: 117-135. Published online: 09 Mar 2024.

  6. Contemporary Educational Psychology

    Contemporary Educational Psychology publishes empirical research from around the globe that substantively advances, extends, or re-envisions the ongoing discourse in educational psychology research and practice. Publishable manuscripts must be grounded in a rich, inclusive theoretical and empirical framework that gives way to critical and timely questions facing educational psychology.

  7. Educational Psychology

    Educational Psychology provides an international forum for the discussion and rapid dissemination of research findings in psychological aspects of education ranging from pre-school to tertiary provision and the education of children with special learning needs. As a journal that focuses on researches within a quantitative, scientific remit, Educational Psychology places particular emphasis on ...

  8. Educational Psychology: Learning and Instruction

    That is, the subsection of learning and instruction within educational psychology traditionally includes multiple theoretical and conceptual domains (e.g., human development, complex cognitive processes, behavioral and social-cognitive views of learning, social-constructivist views of learning, self-regulation and classroom management ...

  9. Frontiers in Psychology

    Research Topics. See all (338) Learn more about Research Topics. This multidisciplinary section explores the psychology and practice of education, drawing on the best science from areas of the social sciences.

  10. British Journal of Educational Psychology

    Online ISSN: 2044-8279. Print ISSN: 0007-0998. The British Journal of Educational Psychology is an international journal for educational and psychological researchers and practitioners that enhances understanding of all aspects of education. We publish work that studies education in formal and informal settings, in students of all ages, that ...

  11. How Scientific Is Educational Psychology Research? The ...

    The field of educational psychology has been moving away from intervention and experimental methods and toward observational and correlational methods (Hsieh et al., Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 523-529, 2005; Reinhart et al., Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(1), 241-247, 2013; Robinson et al., American Educational Research Journal, 44(2), 400-413, 2007). Additionally ...

  12. 66648 PDFs

    This article presents a literature review on reports about people with autism in research carried out in the areas of Education, Psychology, Medicine, and Nursing. We collected twenty-two articles ...

  13. Educational Psychology Review

    Educational Psychology Review is an international forum for the publication of peer-reviewed integrative review articles, special thematic issues, reflections or comments on previous research or new research directions, interviews, and research-based advice for practitioners - all pertaining to the field of educational psychology. The contents provide breadth of coverage appropriate to a wide ...

  14. Trends and Developments in Educational Psychology:

    Brophy, J.E. (1983) 'Research on the Self-fulfilling Prophecy and Teacher Expectations', Journal of Educational Psychology 75: 631-661. Google Scholar Brown, A. and Campione, J.C. (1986) 'Psychological Theory and the Study of Learning Disabilities', American Psychologist 1059-1068.

  15. How peer relationships affect academic achievement among junior high

    Background Despite the recognition of the impact of peer relationships, learning motivation, and learning engagement on academic achievement, there is still a gap in understanding the specific mechanisms through which peer relationships impact academic achievement via learning motivation and learning engagement. Methods This study aims to investigate how peer relationships affect junior high ...

  16. Educational Psychology News -- ScienceDaily

    Educational Psychology News. May 12, 2024. ... Mar. 25, 2024 — Research has often focused on how teachers and educators can best instruct and accommodate students with disabilities. However, are ...

  17. Full article: Making insights from educational psychology and

    The five lenses on online learning in this issue—community, engagement, pedagogy, equity, and design-based research—reveal the diversity and complexity of online teaching and learning as practiced today (Greenhow et al., Citation 2022/this issue).These articles address the fundamental challenge of integrating conceptually driven psychological research and empirically driven educational ...

  18. An exploration into the causal relationships between educational

    npj Mental Health Research - An exploration into the causal relationships between educational attainment, intelligence, and wellbeing: an observational and two-sample Mendelian randomisation study ...

  19. Home

    Overview. Educational Psychology Review is an international forum for the publication of peer-reviewed integrative review articles, special thematic issues, reflections or comments on previous research or new research directions, interviews, and research-based advice for practitioners - all pertaining to the field of educational psychology.

  20. Improving Students' Learning With Effective Learning Techniques

    Many students are being left behind by an educational system that some people believe is in crisis. Improving educational outcomes will require efforts on many fronts, but a central premise of this monograph is that one part of a solution involves helping students to better regulate their learning through the use of effective learning techniques.

  21. Educational Psychology

    All Articles in Educational Psychology. Follow Faceted Search. 7,740 full-text articles. Page 1 of 284. "In My Blood": External Factors For International Stem Postdoctoral Scholars' Career Decisions , Kathryn J. Watson, Sylvia L. Mendez 2024 The University of Iowa. Adaptations And Transformations: Perceptions Of Change In Academic ...

  22. McElroy-Heltzel's research among International Journal of Psychology's

    Her article titled "Resource loss and mental health during COVID-19: Psychosocial protective factors among US older adults and those with chronic disease," published in the International Journal of Psychology, has been listed among the top 10 most-cited papers from January 1, 2022 through December 31, 2023.

  23. How to Get Started on Your First Psychology Experiment

    Even a Little Bit of Expertise Can Go a Long Way. My usual approach to helping students get past this floundering stage is to tell them to avoid thinking up a study altogether. Instead, I tell ...

  24. Free APA Journal Articles

    Recently published articles from subdisciplines of psychology covered by more than 90 APA Journals™ publications. For additional free resources (such as article summaries, podcasts, and more), please visit the Highlights in Psychological Research page. Browse and read free articles from APA Journals across the field of psychology, selected by ...

  25. Ethics, values and Values Based Practice in educational psychology

    Introduction. Lunt and Major (Citation 2000) called for 'the development of a robust epistemology of practice to enable Educational Psychologists (EPs) to articulate their practice' (p. 243).In response, Moore (Citation 2005) appealed for a critical examination of the theoretical foundations of expert practice in educational psychology resulting from the complexities and changes in ...

  26. How to Become a Counseling Psychologist in 2024

    Becoming a counseling psychologist involves considerable investment and can take many years of education and training. For instance, according to 2023 data from the National Science Foundation, it took students an average of 9.1 years since earning their bachelor's degree to get their doctorate in counseling and applied psychology.